• No results found

Increasing perseverance in math: Evidence from a field experiment in Norway

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Share "Increasing perseverance in math: Evidence from a field experiment in Norway"

Copied!
15
0
0

Laster.... (Se fulltekst nå)

Fulltekst

(1)

ContentslistsavailableatScienceDirect

Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization

jo u r n al ho me p ag e :ww w . e l s e v i e r . c o m / l o c a t e / j e b o

Increasing perseverance in math: Evidence from a field experiment in Norway

Eric Bettinger

a,c

, Sten Ludvigsen

b,c

, Mari Rege

c,∗

, Ingeborg F. Solli

c

, David Yeager

c,d

aStanfordUniversity,UnitedStates

bStenLudvigsen,UniversityofOslo,Norway

cUniversityofStavanger,Norway

dUniversityofTexas-Austin,UnitedStates

a rt i c l e i n f o

Articlehistory:

Received13December2016

Receivedinrevisedform18August2017 Accepted30November2017

Availableonline5December2017

Keywords:

Non-cognitiveskills Realeffortexperiment Growthmindset Perseverance Fieldexperiment Education

a b s t ra c t

Researchbypsychologistsandeconomistsdemonstratesthatmanynon-cognitiveskills aremalleableinbothchildrenandadolescents,butwehavelimitedknowledgeonwhat schoolscandotofostertheseskills.Inafieldexperimentrequiringrealeffort,weinvestigate howschoolscanincreasestudents’perseveranceinmathbyshapingstudents’beliefsin theirabilitiestolearn,aconceptreferredtobypsychologistsas“mindset.”Usingprotocols adaptedfrompsychology,weexperimentallymanipulatestudents’beliefsintheirability tolearn.Threeweeksafterourtreatment,wefindpersistenttreatmenteffectsonstudents’

perseveranceandacademicperformanceinmath.Wheninvestigatingsubsamples,wefind thatstudents,whopriortotheexperimenthadlessofabeliefintheirabilitytolearn, generatethetreatmenteffect.Thefindingssuggestthatalow-costinterventionfocusedon students’mindsetcanimprovestudents’engagementandperformance.

©2017ElsevierB.V.Allrightsreserved.

1. Introduction

Non-cognitiveskills,suchasself-controlandperseverance,predictsuccessineducationandinlabormarkets(Borghans etal.,2008;Heckmanetal.,2006;Robertsetal.,2007).Whileresearchersarestilltryingtounderstandthecausalmechanisms, thereisstrongevidencethatindividualswithhighernon-cognitiveskillsaremorelikelytograduatefromhighschool,have higherratesofcollegeattendanceandcompletion,higherwagesandbetteremployment,andevenbetterhealthoutcomes (Carneiroetal.,2007;Kautzetal.,2014).Moreover,researchbypsychologistsandeconomistsdemonstratesthatmany non-cognitiveskillsaremalleable(Alanetal.,2016;Durlaketal.,2011;Kautzetal.,2014).Still,however,wehavelimited knowledgeonwhatschoolscandotofostertheseskills.

Whilenon-cognitiveskillsmaybeacquiredthroughmotivationorself-regulationprogramsrelyingondirectinstruction andrepetitivepractice,meta-analysesfindthatthisapproachhasmixedresults.Itworksreasonablywellwithyoungchildren,

WearegratefultoElinSvensenintheRogalandCountyschooldistrictwhohasfacilitatedtheRCTandgivenvaluablesuggestionsregardingintervention designandimplementation.WearealsogratefultoLeighLauritzenandNettop-UiSformakingthecomputerprogramforthefieldexperiment,andtothe principal,teachersandstudentsatthehighschoolatwhichthefieldexperimentwasimplemented.Finally,weacknowledgetheRCN(227004and260407) andtheNorwegianMinistryofEducationforfunding.

Correspondingauthorat:UniversityofStavanger,4036Stavanger,Norway.

E-mailaddress:mari.rege@uis.no(M.Rege).

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2017.11.032 0167-2681/©2017ElsevierB.V.Allrightsreserved.

(2)

butshowsweakornulleffectswithadolescents(Durlaketal.,2011;HeckmanandKautz,2013;Yeageretal.,inpress).An alternativeapproachtodevelopingnon-cognitiveskills,testedhere,focusesonstudents’beliefs.Ratherthanthinkingof perseveranceasafixedability,wethinkofperseveranceasaseriesofrepeatedchoices.Wedefineastudentwithhigh perseveranceassomeonewhoconsistentlychoosestoexerthigheffort–shestaysfocusedonchallengingtasks,works hard,anddoesnotgiveup.Asachoice,perseveranceshouldbesensitivetobeliefsaboutthepayoffofeffort.Therefore, changingbeliefsabouteffort–inawaythatlastsovertime–mightresultinchangesinperseveranceevenafterarelatively minimalbelief-changeintervention.

Anextensiveliteratureinpsychologyandlaboreconomicshasdemonstratedthatstudents’beliefs,suchasself-efficacy, locusofcontrolorgrit,predicttheirperseveranceandchoices(AlanandErtac,2015;Bandura,1977;Cobb-Clark,2015;Cobb- ClarkandSchurer,2013;ColemanandDeLeire,2003;Duckworthetal.,2007;DuckworthandSeligman,2005).Although informative,thisresearchiscorrelational.Ouridentificationstrategyistorandomlyassignstudentstoawell-validated psychologicalinterventionthatisknowntochangebeliefsabouteffort:agrowthmindsetintervention(Dweck2006;Yeager andDweck2012),alsoreferredtoasan“incrementaltheoryofintelligence”intervention(DweckandLeggett,1988).Growth mindsetinterventionsshapestudents’beliefsintheirabilitytolearn,andcauselastingimprovementsinschooloutcomes (Aronsonetal.,2002;Blackwelletal.,2007;Goodetal.,2003;Pauneskuetal.,2015;Yeageretal.,2016).Intheinterventions studentslearnaboutthebrain’spotentialtogrowandchangethroughdedication,hardworkonchallengingtasks,finding therightlearningstrategies,andseekingassistancefromothers.Thegrowthmindsetinterventionisdesignedtocounteract the“fixedmindset,”whichisthebeliefthatintelligenceortalentsarefixedtraits.Bycontrast,studentswitha“growth mindset”believethattheirabilitiescanbedeveloped.

Inafieldexperimentrequiringrealeffort,weinvestigatehowschoolscanincreasestudents’perseveranceinmathby shapingstudents’beliefsintheirabilitiestolearnorintheirpotentialtobenefitfromeffort.Werelyontheweb-based mindsetinterventioninYeageretal.(2016),whichwasbasedonpriorworkbyAronsonetal.(2002)andPauneskuetal.

(2015).WeadaptedtheinterventiontotheNorwegianlanguage,cultureandcontext.Thetreatmentconditionexposes studentstogrowthmindsetthroughonlinereadingandwritingexercises.Theseexercisesfocuson(1)howintellectual abilitiesaremalleableandaccordinglyhowthebraincangrowandchange;(2)howhardworkonchallengingexercises improvestheneuralconnectionsinone’sbrain;(3)howgrowthmindsetaidsincopingwithconfusionanddifficulty;and (4)howgrowthmindsetcanbeusedtostrivetowardpersonalgoals,givingmorepurposetoexerteffortindifficulttasks (Yeageretal.,2014).Thecontrolconditionhasanalogousactivities,whichteachstudentsfactsaboutmemoryandbrain functioning,butdoesnotaddressthemalleabilityofintellectualability.

Inthespringof2016,385Norwegianhighschoolstudentsparticipatedinthefieldexperiment.Duringnormalschool instructionaltime,weintroducedstudentstoawebsite.Eachstudentloggedinindividuallytothewebsiteonpersonal laptops.Oncestudentsloggedin,oursoftwarerandomlyassignedthemtoeitherthemindsetorcontrolconditions.The studentsworkedontheonlinereadingandwritingexercisesduringtwosessionsof45min,twoweeksapart.Inthethird session,wegavestudentsarealefforttask,designedtocaptureafamiliarschoolactivity,inwhichperseveranceiscriticalto learnandsucceed.Specifically,studentsreceivedaseriesof34multiple-choicealgebraquestionssequentially.Thealgebra questionswerechallenging,possiblymakingmanystudentsfrustratedandtemptedtogiveup.Wetoldthestudentsthat wewouldlikethemto“dotheirbest”andthatthey“maylearnsomethingfromworkingonthemathquestions.”Wealso explainedcarefullythatstudents’answerswouldbekeptconfidentialandthattheirperformancewouldnotaffecttheir grade.Asmanystudentsdidnothavetimetofinishallthequestions,or“gaveup”–clickingfastthroughthequestions– welookathowmanycorrectanswersstudentshadonthefirst10,20andall34questions.

Theexperimentalresultsdemonstratethattreatedstudentshavesignificantlymorecorrect answersonthefirst10 questionscomparedtostudentsinthecontrolgroup(19percentofthestandarddeviation),butthereisnosignificant differencebetweentreatedandcontrolwhenlookingatthefirst20orall34questions.Subsampleanalysisdemonstrates thatthetreatmenteffectisentirelydrivenbystudentswhoinitiallyhadafixedmindset.Forstudentwhoenteredthe experimentwithapre-existingfixedmindset,thetreatmenteffectislargeandsignificantforallthreeoutcomemeasures;

treatedstudentsscored35,34and29percentofastandarddeviationhigherthancontrolstudentsonthefirst10,20andall 34questions,respectively.Forstudentswhoalreadyhadagrowthmindsetpre-treatment,therearenosignificanttreatment effects.Ourdescriptivestatisticsdemonstratethatpriortotheexperiment,studentswithlowgradepointaverage(GPA) andstudentsinvocationaltrackshavelessofagrowthmindset.Investigatingthesesubsampleswefindlargeandsignificant treatmenteffects.Amongstudentsinvocationaltracks,thetreatmentincreasedthescoreonall34questionsby25percent ofastandarddeviation.Ourresultsareconsistentwiththehypothesisthatitispossibletoincreasestudents’perseverance inmathbyshapingtheirbeliefsintheirabilitiestolearn,inparticularamongstudentswhoinitiallyhadafixedmindset.

Thispaperrelatestoseveralstrandsofliteratureineconomics.First,asnotedabove,ourworkbuildsontheemerging literatureonnon-cognitiveskillsbyinvestigatingwhetherschoolscanfosterstudents’perseverancebyshapingtheirbeliefs intheirabilitiestolearn.Thisisimportantbecausethestudyoffersanidentificationstrategyfortestingtheeffectofnon- cognitiveskillsonlifeoutcomes.Second,ourworkalsorelatestorecentdevelopmentsinbehavioraleconomicsofeducation (Kochetal.,2015;Lavecchiaetal.,2014)whichattempttounderstandhowlow-costbehavioralorpsychologicalinter- ventionscanhelpstudentsbetterutilizethelearningopportunitiesalreadywithintheeducationalsystem(e.g.Bettinger etal.,2012;Carrelletal.,2016;CastlemanandPage,2015).Wecontributetothisliteraturebyinvestigatinghowabrief, low-costpsychologicalinterventioncanleadstudentstoincreasetheireffortinalearningtaskthreeweekslater.Third,our mindsetexperimentbuildsonotherbehavioraleconomicsexperimentsdesignedtounderstandindividual’smotivationand

(3)

performanceinrealeffortchoicesinthelaborfield(AzmatandIriberri,2010;Bradleretal.,2016;Erikssonetal.,2009;Koch etal.,2015;Kvaløyetal.,2015).

Finally,ourexperimentcontributestothepsychologicalliteratureonmindset.Thepsychologicalmechanismsformindset effectsareclearlydefinedandstronglyillustratedinlaboratoryexperiments:growthmindsetinterventionschangestudents’

effortbeliefs,theirattributions,andtheirgoals(Burnetteetal.,2013).YetasarguedbyWilsonandButtrick(2016)andMiller etal.(2017),thebehavioralmechanismsthatexplainhowachangeinbeliefscantranslateintoachangeingradesmonthsor yearslater,arenotfullydocumented.Thismakes“longlag”interventioneffectsseem“magical”(YeagerandWalton,2011).

Weshinesomelightintothisbehavioral“blackbox”,bydemonstratingthatthemindsetinterventionaffectsperseverance inarealeffortlearningtaskwhichtookplacethreeweeksaftertheintervention.Whileourstudyfocusesonmindset,it buildsupontheextantliteraturedemonstratingthatbehavioraltrainingcaninfluenceacademicoutcomes(e.g.Alanetal., 2016;DuckworthandSeligman,2005;Durlaketal.,2011).

2. Background

2.1. Sampleandinstitutionalcontext

TheparticipantsinourexperimentareNorwegianfirstyearhighschoolstudentsinRogalandCounty.WefocusonNorway forthreedistinctreasons.First,todate,theexistingresearchonmindsetfocusesontheUnitedStates.Usingasampleoutside theUnitedStatescanadvanceunderstandingofmindset.ThecapabilityoftheNorwegiangovernmenttotrackstudents throughouttheirlifetime,givesthepossibilitytomeasurethelong-runimpactsofgrowthmindsets.Second,Norway,like manycountries,divideshighschoolstudentsintovocationalandacademictracks.Thistrackingisoftencorrelatedwithprior achievement,andasweshowbelow,thevocationalstudentsinoursamplehavesignificantlylowerincidenceofgrowth mindsets.Third,oursampleisalsodrawnoutofconvenience.MultiplecoauthorsliveintheRogalandregionofNorwayand haveexistingpartnershipsinimplementingeducationalinterventions.Atthetimeoftheresearch,educationalpolicymakers throughoutNorwaywerediscussingtheacademicresearchongrowthmindsetanditsimplicationsforNorway.

InNorway,students starthighschoolaroundage16 aftertenyearsofcompulsoryschoolingsimilarforeverybody (primaryandmiddleschool).1Astudentcanapplytoanyhighschoolinhercounty.Whenapplyingastudenthastodecide whethertoenrollinavocationaltrack,whichleadsdirectlytoemployment,oranacademictrack,whichpreparesstudents toattendcollegeafterhighschoolcompletion.Studentsrankthreedesiredchoices,andacceptanceisbasedonstudents’

GPAfrommiddleschool.Allstudentsareguaranteedacceptanceintoahighschoolinthecounty.

Norwegianhighschooltypicallylastthreetofouryearsdependingonthespecificprogram.Only70percentofstudents completehighschoolwithinfiveyears.Forvocationaltrackstudents,completionratesareparticularlylow–only55percent completewithinfiveyears.WhileNorwegianpolicymakershaveaimedseveralreformsatimprovinghighschoolcompletion rates,therehavebeennosystematiceffortstoalterstudents’beliefsintheirabilitiestolearnasameansofimproving educationaloutcomes.

2.2. Conceptualframework

Wedefineastudentwithhighperseveranceassomeonewhoconsistentlyexertshigheffort–shestaysfocusedon task,workshardonchallengingyetpotentiallyrewardingtasks,anddoesnotgiveup.Belowwepresentastylizedmodel illustratinghowshapingstudents’beliefsintheirabilitiestolearncanaffecttheirperseverance.

Consideralearningsituation.Astudentchooseshowmuchefforttoexert.Bystayingfocusedontask,workinghardand notgivingup,thestudentcanbenefitfromlearning.However,theremaybeanopportunitycostofexertinghigheffort–for example,thestudentcannotcheckherphone,daydream,ortalktoaclassmate.

LetpB(e) representthestudent’sexpectedbenefitoflearningwhenexertingeffort,e,whereB>0,B<0andpisthe student’sbeliefinherabilitiestolearn,i.e.howefforttranslateintobenefitoflearning.LetC(e)representtheopportunity costofexertingeffort,whereC>0andC>0.Autilitymaximizingstudentchooseseffortlevele*suchthatC(e)=p·B(e).

Fromthisfirstordercondition,itfollowsthat dedp >0andd2e

d2p <0.Thus,wehavethefollowingtwoconjectures:

Conjecture1. Itispossibletoincreaseastudent’seffortbyincreasingherbeliefinherabilitytolearn.

Conjecture2. Theeffort-effectofincreasingthestudent’sbeliefinherabilitiestolearnishigher,thelowerthestudent’s initialbelief.

2.3. Academicmindsetinterventions

Inpsychology,astudent’sbeliefinherabilitiestolearnisreferredtoasheracademicmindset(Dweck,2006).Students witha“fixedmindset”believetheirintelligenceortalentsarefixedtraits.Studiesusingsurveymeasuresofmindsetsand

1About9percentofstudentsinoursamplewereolderthan16,meaningthattheyeitherdelayedkindergartenentry,experiencedgraderepetitionat somepointintheiracademiccareer,or“tooktimeoff”aftercompulsoryschooling.

(4)

Fig.1. ContentofComputerProgram.

experimentalmanipulationsofmindsetsinlaboratoryandschoolsettingssuggestthatafixedmindsetshapesstudents’

academicachievementsinmanyways(Dweck2006;YeagerandDweck2012;YeagerandWalton,2011).First,students withafixedmindsetavoidacademicchallenges.Theywanteasierproblemsthatwillmakethemlookandfeelsmart(Mueller andDweck1998;Yeageretal.,2016).Second,afixedmindsetleadstounproductivebeliefsaboutefforts.Forexample,a studentwithafixedmindsetmightsay“IfIhavetotryhardatmath,I’mnotsmartatmath”(Blackwelletal.,2007).Last, fixed-mindsetstudentsarelessresilient.Instead,theyhidesetbacksanddeficiencies,notwantingpeopletoseethemas havinglowability.Theyfailtoaskforhelpandsometimesevenlieaboutlowscores(MuellerandDweck,1998).

Bycontrast,students with“growthmindsets”believethat intelligencecangrowand improveinresponsetoeffort, goodstrategies,andhelpfromothers.Fromthisperspective,anacademicchallengeisnotathreattoone’sability;itisan opportunityforlearningandimprovement.Inagrowthmindset,effortisagoodthing:astudentmightsay“tryingharder makesyousmarter”(Blackwelletal.,2007).Inthefaceofadifficultproblem,agrowthmindsetstudentismoreresilient, seeksappropriatehelp,orswitchesstrategies.Thestudentdoesnothideconfusion.Comparedtoafixedmindset,holdinga growthmindsetpredictsmorelearning,betterlearningstrategies,andhighergradesovertime,providedthattheschoolwork ischallenging(Blackwelletal.,2007;YeagerandDweck2012).

Thereissubstantialevidencesuggestingthatparentsandteacherssocializechildren’smindsetsthrougheverydaycom- munication(KaminsandDweck1999;MuellerandDweck1998;Rattanetal.,2015).Subtleverbalfeedbackfromadults canputchildreninafixedmindsetandundermineinternalmotivation.Thiscanhappenevenfromvaluedcaregiverstrying toencouragechildren.Forexample,aclassicpaperbyMuellerandDweck(1998)showedthatpraisingyoungadolescents fortheirintelligence–tellingthemtheywere“smart”whentheydidwell−createdafixedmindsetandunderminedtheir resilienceinthefaceoflaterstruggle.Incontrast,praisingstudents’“processes”(effortsorstrategies)putchildrenina growthmindsetandfosteredresilience.

Recentlyseveralstudieshavedemonstratedthatprecisetheory-basedinterventionscancommunicateagrowthmindset toyouthsandproducelastingimprovementsinstudents’grades(Aronsonetal.,2002;Blackwelletal.,2007;Goodetal., 2003;Pauneskuetal.,2015;Yeageretal.,2016).Theseinterventionsappealtoneuroscienceandevidenceonthemalleability ofthebrain.Tocommunicatethemalleabilityofintelligence,theseinterventionsusephysicalexerciseasametaphorfor growthmindset.Theinterventionsteachthestudentstothinkoftheirbrainsasmuscles,whichgetstrongerasoneexercises them.Theinterventiondepictsnewneuronalconnectionsgrowingasstudentscompletechallengingmathproblems.

3. Experimentaldesign 3.1. Interventionandmeasures

Wedevelopacomputerprogramwiththreeonlinesessions,eachlastingabout45min.Webasecontentandvisuallayout inSessions1and2ontheinterventioninYeageretal.(2016)(alsoseePauneskuetal.,2015).However,bythemeansofa professionaltranslatorandinterviewswithseveralfocusgroupsofNorwegianhighschoolstudents,wecarefullyadapted thematerialtotheNorwegianlanguage,cultureandcontext.Session3consistsofarealefforttaskinwhichthestudents havetosolveaseriesofalgebraquestions.Fig.1illustratesthecontentofthethreesessions.

InSession1studentsfirstanswersurveyquestionsdesignedtomeasurestudents’mindsetsatbaseline.2Inparticular, weaskhowmuch,onascalefrom1to6,thestudentdisagrees(1)oragrees(6)withthefollowingstatements(assigned variablenameinparenthesis):

2 Below,weonlylistsurveyquestionsusedforthispaper.Thestudentsreceivedothersurveyitems,designedtoanswerdifferentresearchquestions.

Allsurveyquestionswereidenticalfortreatedandcontrol.

(5)

Fig.2.ScreenShotfromComputerProgram.

•“Youhaveacertainamountofintelligence,andyoureallycan’tdomuchtochangeit”(FixedMindset1);

•“Yourintelligenceissomethingaboutyouthatyoucan’tchangeverymuch”(FixedMindset2);

•“Beinga‘mathperson’ornotissomethingthatyoureallycan’tchange.Somepeoplearegoodatmathandotherpeople aren’t”(FixedMindsetMath);and

•“Whenyouhavetotryreallyhardinasubjectinschool,itmeansyoucan’tbegoodatthatsubject”(FixedMindsetEffort).

Thesemindsetmeasureshavebeenusedandvalidatedinnumerousstudies,demonstratingthattheystronglypredict gradesandperformanceonbehavioraltasks(seee.g.Burnetteetal.,2013;Yeageretal.,2016)

Afterrespondingtothesurveyquestions,thestudentsreceivetheintervention.Thecomputerprogramrandomlyallocates studentstoeitherthetreatmentorcontrolconditions.Thetreatedstudentshavetodothreecognitiveexercises.First, studentshavetoreadanarticleaboutresearchinneurosciencethatdemonstratesthebrain’spotentialtogrowandchange, originallywrittenfortheexperimentinBlackwelletal.(2007)andsubstantiallyrevisedinYeageretal.(2016).Thearticle presentationrunsoverseveralscreens(oneofwhichappearsina singlescreenshotinFig.2)andhasastylizedvisual layoutwithillustrations.Italsousesthemetaphorthatthebrainislikeamusclethatgrowsinresponsetochallenging learningexperiences.Second,studentsareaskedtosummarizethearticleandexplainhowitsmessagerelatestotheirown lives.Linkinginformationtotheselfinthiswaymakesitmoreself-relevantandeasiertorecall(BowerandGilligan,1979;

HullemanandHarackiewicz,2009).Third,studentsareaskedwhatgrowthmindsetadvicetheymightgivetoafriendwho wasstrugglinginschool.Providingadvicetoothersisa“saying-is-believing”tactictoencouragestudentstointernalizethe ideasbyendorsingthemtosomeoneelse(seee.g.Aronsonetal.,2002).

Studentsinthecontrolcondition,likethoseinthetreatmentcondition,readabriefarticleaboutthebrainandanswer reflectivequestions.However,theydonotlearnaboutthebrain’smalleability.Instead,theylearnaboutbasicbrainfunctions andtheirlocalization,forexample,thekeyfunctionsassociatedwitheachcorticallobe.Theexperimentalconditionsare designedtolookverysimilartodiscouragestudentsfromcomparingtheirmaterials.Itinvolvesthesametypeofgraphic art–e.g.imagesofthebrain,animations−aswellascompellingstories.

InSession2,theinterventionusesinsightsfromthesocialpsychologyofattitudechangeandpersuasion,whatwecall

“supportivepsychologies”,toallowabrief,onlineinterventiontoachievelastingeffects.Thepurposeofthe“supportive psychologies”istocreateagestaltimpressionthatthegrowthmindsetmessageismemorable,credible,normal,andimpor- tant.Specifically,Session2repeatsthemetaphorfromSession1,thatthebrainislikeamusclethatgrowsinresponse tochallengingtasks(memorable);Itincludesquotesfromscientistsandcelebritieswhoendorsedthenotionthatthebrain developswhenitlearnsandexplainedhowastrongerbraincouldleadtoimprovementsinpersonalandsocialwelfare(cred- ible);Itleveragessocialnormsbyincludingquotationsfrompastparticipantsendorsingtheprogrammessages(Cialdiniand Goldstein,2004)(normal);Finally,Session2emphasizesapurposeforlearning(Yeageretal.,2014)byincludingprosocial

(6)

beyond-the-selfmotivesforadoptingandusingagrowthmindset(Grant2013;Yeageretal.,2016)(important).Forexample, onescreenintheinterventionreadsasfollows:“Peopletellusthattheyareexcitedtolearnaboutagrowthmindsetbecause ithelpsthemachievethegoalsthatmattertothemandtopeopletheycareabout.Theyusethemindsettolearninschool sotheycangivebacktothecommunityandmakeadifferenceintheworldlater.”

Inadditiontoreinforcingthebriefmindsetintervention,the“supportivepsychologies”areimportantforthefollowing reason:Thisstudyisoneofthefirstpapersineconomicsfocusingonmindset.Oneofourgoalsistoreplicatemindsetas truetotheoriginalpsychologyliteratureaspossible.Somepriorworkbyeconomists(e.g.Dee2014)hasfailedtoreplicate psychologicalinterventionspossiblyinpartbecauseofsubstantialdeviationsfromthepsychologicalintervention.Weare relyingontheexactscript(adaptedtoNorwegianlanguageandcontext)ofthelargestandmosteffectivemindsetstudyto date.

The“supportivepsychologies”dopresentsomeissuesintheinterpretationoftheresults.Inparticular,ourintervention inSession2couldaffectacademicperformancethroughsocialpressure:Theuseofcrediblerolemodelsandsocialnorms maycreatesocialpressureforstudentstoadoptgrowthmindsets.Weareunabletodisentanglewhethertheimpactsderive primarilyfromeducationaboutthemind(Session1)orfromthesocialpressureexertedinSession2.Bothsessionspoint studentstowardimprovinggrowthmindset,butwedonotidentifytherelativestrengthoftheeffectfromthetwosessions.

SimilartoSession1,thecontrolstudents’activityinSession2isdesignedtobeparalleltothetreatmentactivity.Students learnmoreaboutthebrain,butnotaboutitsmalleabilityandgrowthmindset.Ingeneral,wetookeveryprecautiontomake surethattherewasminimaltonocontaminationacrosstreatmentcategoriesduringimplementation.Ifsomecontamination occurredafterimplementation(forexampleifstudentstalkedtoeachotherabouttreatmentmaterial),thisislikelytobias ourestimatedimpactsdownward.

Aftertheinterventionmaterial,Session2providesallthestudentswiththesameseriesofsurveyquestions,measuring students’mindsets,asatthestartofSession1.Thereafterweincludeameasureofchallengeseekingwhichpriorresearch hasassociatedwithgrowthmindsets(Blackwelletal.,2007;MuellerandDweck1998).SimilartoYeageretal.(2016)we letstudentscreatetheirownmathworksheetwhichtheywillhavetoworkoninSession3.Studentscanpickfromeasy questionsfromwhichtheylikelywillnotlearnnewskills,orhardquestions,whichmayrequiremoreeffortbutprovide morelearningopportunities.Asmeasuresofstudents’challengeseeking,weusenumberofveryhardquestionsselected andnumberofveryhardorsomewhathardquestionsselected.ThequestionswereprovidedbyTheNorwegianDirectorate forEducationandTraining,andwecategorizedthemintoeasy,hardandveryhardbasedonpreviousstudents’scoreson eachquestion.

Finally,inSession3studentsfirsthavetosolvetworandomlydrawnquestionsfromtheworksheettheycreatedin Session2.3Aftertheworksheetquestions,thestudentshavetoparticipateinarealefforttask,consistingof34multiple choicealgebraquestions,givensequentially.4Thealgebraquestionswerechallenging,andseveralstudentsdidnothave timetoworkonallthequestions.Onaverage,thestudentsansweredcorrectly45,41,and37percentofthefirst10,20and 34questions,respectively.

Theon-screenintroductiontothealgebraquestionstellsthestudentsthattheywillbegivenaseriesofalgebraquestions, andthattheyshouldtrytodotheirbesttofindthecorrectanswer.Moreover,itexplainsthatstudentsmightlearnsomething fromworkingonthemathquestions,butthattheirperformancewillnotaffecttheirgrade.Asmanystudentsgaveuporran outoftimeonthelastquestions,weusenumberofcorrectquestionsonthefirst10,20,andall34questionsasmeasuresof students’effortonthealgebraquestions.

Notably,thestudentsdidnotknowthattheywouldreceivealgebraquestionsinSession3,sotherewasnowaytoprepare.

Moreover,algebrawasnotonthecurriculuminschoolbetweenSession1andSession3.Thus,ifwefindatreatmenteffect onthesemeasures,itisreasonabletointerpretitasaneffectofeffort(studentsaremorefocusedontask,workharderand donotgiveup)andnotthattreatedstudentshaveactuallybecomebetterinalgebra(althoughwecannotruleoutthatsome studentsmayhavebeensufficientlymotivatedtopursueoutsidelearningexperience).

Apossibleconcernwithourrealefforttaskisdemandinducedeffects,thatstudentsintheexperimentalcondition increaseefforttocomplywiththeexperimenters’wishes,andnotbecausetheyhavemoreofagrowthmindset.Tolimit thispossibility,weprovidenopromptsbeforepresentingthetestquestionsinSession3,andtheformatofthesessionhas minimalremindersofthepriorsessions:Experimenterswerenotpresentintheclassroom,andthegraphicsonthetest questionsdidnotreiteratetheexperiment.Furthermore,effectscausedbyachangeinmindsetareexpectedtodifferacross subsamples(seeHypothesis2below),whereastherearenoclearreasontoexpectdifferentialdemandinducedeffects.

Asasecondarymatter,ourdataalsoallowustoinvestigatetimespentoneachalgebraquestion.Ontheonehand,we couldimaginethatstudentswithmoreperseverancemanagetostaymorefocusedandworkharderandhenceareableto movefasterthroughtheproblems.Ontheotherhand,giventhedifficultyofthequestions,studentswithmoreperseverance mayhavespentmoretimetryingtosolveaquestionbeforegivingup.Theymayhaveelectedtotrydifferentapproaches

3 Wedidnotusethisdata,asselectioninSession2(treatmentaffectschallengeseeking)mayaffectperformanceonthesequestions.However,giving thestudentstimeinSession3,toworkonsomequestionsfromtheworksheet,isstillanimportantpartofthedesigntoavoiddeceptioninSession2.

4 Thefirst13questionswerethesameforallstudents.Thereafter,thecomputerprogramrandomlyassignedthestudentstooneofthreegroups,and eachgroupreceivedtheremainingalgebraquestionsindifferentorder.ThequestionswereselectedfromthepubliclyreleasedNAEPtestquestionsand theCaliforniaStandardsTestsquestions.

(7)

insteadofmakingarandomguessandmovingontothenextquestion.Assuch,wedonothaveaclearhypothesis,asto howourinterventionaffectedtimeuse.Wheninvestigatingtimespentonthefirst10,20or32min(resultsavailablefrom authorsonrequest),wefindnosignificanttreatmenteffectsneitheronthefullsamplenoronrelevantsubsamples.

3.2. Hypotheses

RecallfromthestylizedmodelinSection2.2,ifthetreatmentincreasesastudent’sbeliefinherabilitiestolearn,this increaseshermarginalbenefitofeffort,andleadstoanincreaseintheoptimaleffortlevelinalearningsituation(Conjecture 1).Assuch,wehypothesize:

Hypothesis1. Thetreatmenthasapositiveeffectonastudent’seffortintherealefforttaskinSession3.

Thestylizedmodelalsodemonstratedthatanincreaseinastudent’sbeliefinherabilitiestolearn,increasesoptimal effortlevelatadiminishingrate(Conjecture2).Assuch,wehypothesize:

Hypothesis2. Treatmenteffectsoneffortintherealefforttaskarelargerforstudentswhoinitiallyhavelowbeliefsin theirabilitiestolearn.

Hypothesis2isalsoconsistentwithseveralstudiesdemonstratingthatinitiallylowperformingstudentsbenefitmore frommindsetinterventions,andthatafixedmindsetismorecommonamongthesestudentspriortotheintervention (Pauneskuetal.,2015;Yeageretal.,2016).

4. Sampleandprocedures

Inthespringof2016,allfirstyearstudentsatapublichighschoolinruralNorwayparticipatedinthefieldexperiment.

Astheschoolservesalargeregion,itislargeandoffersbothavocationalandacademictrack.Participationwasmandatory aspartoftheschoolinstruction,butstudents hadtoconsenttoparticipateintheresearchproject.Whenthestudent loggedontothefirstsession,theyreceivedinformationabouttheresearchprojectandhadtomaketheirconsentdecision.

Wehad458studentsparticipateinthefirstsession,amongwhom385studentsconsentedtoparticipateintheresearch project.

Afterastudenthadmadetheconsentdecision,werandomlyassignedthestudenttoeitherthemindset(treatment)or controlcondition.Amongthestudentswhoconsented,22studentshadmissingregistrydataonmiddleschoolgrades.

Another9 studentswere olderthan20 years old.5 Droppingthesestudents fromoursample resultedin aSession 1 sampleof354students.AbsenceisamajorconcerninNorwegianhighschools,andweexperiencedsomeattritionin Sessions2and3.FromourSession1sample,289and254studentsparticipated inSessions2and3,respectively.Our balancetest(seeTable1)demonstratesthatattritionwasnotsignificantlycorrelatedwithtreatmentstatus.Notably,we collecteddataforstudentsinSession3eveniftheyhadnotparticipatedinSession2,i.e.receivedthetreatmentreinforce- ment.

Weimplementedallsessionsinthestudents’classroomduringschoolhours.Thestudentsusedtheirownlaptopcom- putersandheadset.6Atthebeginningofeachsession,wereadabriefscripttoallthestudents.Wetoldthestudentsthat theywereabouttologontoacomputerprogramdesignedtolearnaboutthebrainandreflectonlearning.Weaskedthe studentstoworkindependentlyandnottalktootherstudents.Wealsoemphasizedthatstudentsshoulddotheirbestand thattheiranswerswouldbekeptanonymousandnotaffecttheirgrades.Weassuredthemthattheirteacherorschool wouldneverseetheirindividualanswers.Finally,wetoldthestudentsthatthesessionwouldlastfor45minandprovided themwithlogoninformation.InSession1andSession2,membersofourresearchteamadministeredtheprotocolwiththe teacherpresentintheclassroom.InSession3,theteacherswereresponsibleforimplementation.Weprovidedascriptto them.Membersofourresearchteamwerestillpresentattheschoolincasetheteachershadanyquestionsortechnical challenges.Inallthreesessions,ifstudentsfinishedpriortothe45min,theywereaskedtoworkonotherschoolwork.There arefouropen-endedquestionsintreatedSession1.Thefrequencyofstudentsputtingeffortintotheopen-endedquestions suggestthatmoststudentspaidattentiontothetreatmentmaterial.About85percentansweredsubstantively,whichwas definedasanyattemptofasincereanswer.

Studentsloggedonwithauniquestudentnumberandpasswordassignedtoeachstudentbytheschooldistrictadminis- tration.Teacherswereunawareofstudents’treatmentstatus.Forthestudentswhoconsented,theschooldistrictprovidedus withregistrydatautilizingthesameuniquestudentnumber.Theschooldistrictde-identifiedthedatabeforetheyprovided thedatatoourteam.Fromtheregistrydata,weemploythefollowingvariables:GPAandmathgrade7frommiddleschool,

5Thisimplieslaggingbehindregularschoolprogressionbyatleastfouryears.

6Alaptopismandatoryforschoolwork.Allthestudentshaveschooldistrictsubsidizedlaptops.Wehadsomeextraheadsetstolendtostudentswho didnothaveheadset.

7Sixstudentswereregisteredwithoutamathgrade.Thesemissingobservationswerereplacedbypredictedvalues(predictionbasedonbaselinemindset measures,GPA,genderandvocationaltrack).Thecorrelationbetweenpredictedandobservedvaluesare0.72.Resultsarerobustwithandwithoutthese students.

(8)

Table1

DescriptiveStatisticsandBalanceTest.

Session1 Session2 Session3

Control Treatment Difference Control Treatment Difference Control Treatment Difference

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9)

GPA 0.083 −0.081 −0.165 0.165 0.092 −0.072 0.224 0.141 −0.083

(0.916) (1.072) (0.106) (0.809) (0.899) (0.101) (0.892) (1.029) (0.121)

Mathgrade 0.066 −0.064 −0.130 0.130 0.062 −0.067 0.172 0.141 −0.031

(0.970) (1.026) (0.106) (0.940) (1.005) (0.115) (0.956) (1.029) (0.125)

Vocationaltrack 0.583 0.664 0.082 0.566 0.645 0.080 0.489 0.561 0.073

(0.494) (0.473) (0.051) (0.497) (0.479) (0.057) (0.501) (0.498) (0.063)

Female 0.440 0.525 0.085 0.441 0.548 0.107+ 0.511 0.595 0.084

(0.497) (0.500) (0.053) (0.498) (0.499) (0.059) (0.511) (0.492) (0.062)

Olderthan16 0.091 0.078 −0.013 0.055 0.027 −0.027 0.075 0.057 −0.017

(0.289) (0.269) (0.030) (0.304) (0.164) (−0.023) (0.265) (0.234) (0.031)

FixedMindset1 0.049 −0.047 −0.096 0.008 −0.062 −0.071 0.036 −0.108 −0.144

(0.996) (1.004) (0.106) (0.977) .(983) (0.115) (0.988) (0.993) (0.124)

FixedMindset2 0.041 −0.040 −0.081 0.046 −0.050 −0.097 0.027 −0.047 −0.074

(0.993) (1.008) (0.106) (0.988) (0.988) (0.116) (0.957) (0.979) (0.121)

FixedMindsetMath 0.047 −0.045 −0.092 0.040 −0.109 −0.150 0.029 −0.034 −0.063

(1.013) (0.987) (0.106) (0.974) (0.968) (0.114) (0.968) (0.951) (0.120)

FixedMindsetEffort −0.035 0.034 0.070 −0.111 0.000 0.112 −0.090 0.044 0.135

(0.965) (1.034) (0.106) (0.885) (0.987) (0.110) (0.906) (1.004) (0.121)

BaselineGrowthMindset −0.038 0.036 0.075 0.001 0.079 0.078 −0.005 0.054 0.059

(.1.020) (0.982) (0.106) (0.968) (0.936) (0.112) (0.984) (0.957) (0.122)

N 175 179 354 145 144 289 133 121 254

Notes:Foreachsessionsample,columnsprovidethemean(andstandarddeviation)forthecontrolgroupandthetreatedgroup,andtheestimatedcoefficient (robuststandarderror)fromregressingeachcovariateagainsttreatmentstatus.+p<0.10,*p<0.05,**p<0.01.

Table2

CorrelationbetweenPre-TreatmentMindsetMeasures.

FixedMindset1 FixedMindset2 FixedMathMindset FixedEffortMindset

FixedMindset2 0.690**

FixedMathMindset 0.281** 0.425**

FixedEffortMindset 0.212** 0.322** 0.299**

BaselineGrowthMindset −0.765** −0.845** −0.696** −0.600**

Notes:+p<0.10,*p<0.05,**p<0.01.Session1sample(n=354).

highschooltrack(indicatorforvocational),gender(indicatorforfemale)andage(indicatorforlaggingbehindregularschool progression,i.e.beingolderthan16years).

5. Results

5.1. Balancetestanddescriptivestatistics

Table1presentsourdescriptivestatisticsandbalancetests.ThefourFixedMindsetmeasurescorrespondstothefour surveyquestionsasreportedinSection3.1.Thesevariablesarestandardizedwithmeanzeroandstandarddeviationone usingtheSession1sample,andapositivescoreindicatesafixedmindset.TheBaselineGrowthMindsetmeasureisthe meanofthefourfixedmindsetmetrics,wherethescaleisreversedandstandardized.Hence,apositivescoreindicatesa growthmindset.Fortheeaseofinterpretation,wehavealsostandardizedGPAandmathgradefrommiddleschoolusingthe Session1sample.Olderthan16isanindicatorforlaggingbehindregularschoolprogression.ThecolumnslabeledControl andTreatmentprovidesummarystatisticsforthecontrolandtreatmentgroupineachsession.IntheSession1control group,wecanseethatabout44percentofthestudentsarefemale;58percentareinthevocationaltrack;and9.1percent ofstudentsareoneortwoyearsolderthantheon-trackageof16.Allothercovariatesarestandardized.Whencomparing acrosssessions,thenumbersindicatethattheprobabilityofcompletingallsessionsaresomewhathigherforthosewith ahighGPA,beingonacademictrack,females,andhavingapre-interventiongrowthmindset.Weregresseachcovariate againsttreatmentstatus,andpresenttheresultingcoefficientandrobuststandarderrorincolumnslabeledDifference.We findthattherearesignificantlymorefemalesinthetreatedgroupinSession2(p<0.1),butallothercharacteristicsarewell balancedacrosstreatmentstatus.WeconcludethatrandomizationwassuccessfulandthatattritioninSessions2and3did notleadtosignificantdifferencesintreatmentstatus.

(9)

Table3

PredictorsofBaselineGrowthMindset.

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)

GPA 0.250** 0.139+

(0.052) (0.077)

Mathgrade 0.260** 0.179*

(0.051) (0.075)

Vocationaltrack −0.282* 0.053

(0.109) (0.129)

Female 0.151 0.082

(0.106) (0.105)

Olderthan16 −0.157 0.107

(0.191) (0.193)

R-squared 0.060 0.065 0.016 0.003 0.003 0.066

Notes:+p<0.10,*p<0.05,**p<0.01.Dependentvariable:BaselineGrowthMindset.Eachcolumnpresentsaseparateregressionandreportstheestimated coefficient(robuststandarderror)forallincludedcovariates.Session1sample(n=354).

Table4

TreatmentEffectonPost-TreatmentMindsetandChallengeSeekinginSession2.

Post-TreatmentGrowthMindset Choosing“VeryHard”

ChallengeQuestions

Choosing“Hard”or“Very Hard”ChallengeQuestions

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)

Treatment 0.557** 0.545** 0.234* 0.285** 0.240** 0.300**

(0.113) (0.090) (0.117) (0.110) (0.117) (0.111)

GPA 0.055 0.040 0.119

(0.085) (0.105) (0.105)

Mathgrade 0.228** 0.378** 0.407**

(0.068) (0.084) (0.084)

Vocationaltrack 0.006 0.159 −0.006

(0.114) (0.140) (0.140)

Female 0.002 −0.468** −0.417**

(0.092) (0.113) (0.114)

Olderthan16 0.323 −0.204 −0.094

(0.225) (0.277) (0.277)

BaselineGrowth 0.522** 0.045 0.020

Mindset (0.048) (0.059) (0.059)

R-squared 0.075 0.435 0.010 0.144 0.011 0.141

Notes:+p<0.10,*p<0.05,**p<0.01.Firstrowliststhedependentvariable.Eachcolumnpresentsaseparateregressionandreportstheestimatedcoefficient (robuststandarderror)forallincludedcovariates.Session2sample(n=289).Forcolumns(1)and(2):n=288.

InTable2wepresentacorrelationmatrixofourmindsetmeasurespresentedinSection3.1.FixedMindset18andFixed Mindset2representtwodifferentwordingsofthesamequestion,and itshouldnotbesurprisingthattheyarehighly correlated.Wecanalsoseethatthesedirectmeasuresofafixedmindsetarestronglycorrelatedwithourmeasuresof havingafixedmindsetwhenitcomestoMath(FixedMindsetMath)andEffort(FixedMindsetEffort).Thesearemoreindirect measureswhichmeasuretheconsequencesofhavinglessofagrowthmindset.ThroughoutwewilluseBaselineGrowthas ourpreferredmindsetmeasure.

InTable3weinvestigatehowourpre-treatmentcovariatespredictsagrowthmindset.WecanseeinColumns1–3 thatthepresenceofgrowthmindsetseemstobesignificantlymorelikelyforstudentswithhighGPA/mathgradeandfor studentswhodonotattendvocationaltracks.VocationaltracksinNorwaygenerallyincludestudentswithloweracademic credentials.InColumn5weaddallpredictorstothesamemodelandwefindthatthereisonlyasignificantrelationship betweenGPA/mathgradeandgrowthmindset.

5.2. Treatmenteffects

InTable4weinvestigatetreatmenteffectsonoutcomemeasuresgatheredattheendofSession2.First,weseeifthe treatmentaffectedthemeasureofgrowthmindset.Wegatheredthesamemeasuresofmindsetpost-treatment aswe didatbaseline.Post-TreatmentGrowthMindsetisthenconstructedidenticallytoourBaselineGrowthMindsetvariable,and standardizedontheSession2sample.WecanseefromColumn1thatthereisalargeandsignificanteffectoftreatmenton

8Onestudentdidnotrespondtothefirstfixedmindsetquestion.Thismissingobservationwasreplacedbythepredictedvalue(predictionbasedon baselinemindsetmeasures,GPA,genderandvocationaltrack).Thecorrelationbetweenpredictedandobservedvaluesare0.70.Resultsarerobustwith andwithoutthisstudent.

(10)

Table5

TreatmentEffectonEffortinSession3.

ScoreonFirst10Questions ScoreonFirst20Questions ScoreonAll34Questions

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)

Treatment 0.121 0.193* 0.057 0.134 0.012 0.089

(0.126) (0.089) (0.126) (0.085) (0.126) (0.086)

GPA −0.036 0.004 −0.028

(0.072) (0.068) (0.069)

Mathgrade 0.542** 0.582** 0.551**

(0.067) (0.063) (0.064)

Vocationaltrack −0.643** −0.542** −0.602**

(0.110) (0.103) (0.106)

Female −0.081 −0.134 −0.176*

(0.092) (0.086) (0.088)

Olderthan16 0.492** 0.464** 0.304+

(0.184) (0.172) (0.176)

BaselineGrowth 0.021 0.068 0.102*

Mindset (0.048) (0.045) (0.046)

R-squared 0.000 0.506 0.003 0.567 0.004 0.544

Notes:+p<0.10,*p<0.05,**p<0.01.Firstrowliststhedependentvariable.Eachcolumnpresentsaseparateregressionandreportstheestimatedcoefficient (robuststandarderror)forincludedcovariates.Allincludedcovariatesarereported,exceptfromincolumns2,4and6whereweadditionallycontrolfor questionassignmentorder(twoindicators).Session3sample(n=254).

growthmindset.Indeed,treatmentincreasesthescoreby56percentofastandarddeviation.InColumn2wecanseethat thisfindingisrobusttocontrollingforourpre-interventionvariables,includingbaselinegrowthmindset.

Incolumns3–6,weinvestigatehowtreatmentaffectedchallengeseekingwhenstudentshadtocreatetheirownmath worksheetinSession2.AsexplainedinSection3.1,challengeseekingismeasuredasthenumberof“veryhard”or“somewhat orveryhard”questionschosen.Thechallengeseekingmeasuresarestandardizedwithmeanzeroandstandarddeviation oneusingtheSession2-sample.InColumn3,weseethattreatedstudentschosesignificantlymore“veryhard”questions comparedtostudentsinthecontrolgroup.Wecanseethattheestimateisrobusttocontrollingforourpre-intervention variablesinColumn4.Itsuggeststhattreatmentincreaseschallengeseekingby29percentofastandarddeviation.In Columns5and6,weusenumberof“somewhathard”or“veryhard”questionsastheoutcomemeasure,andwegetasimilar treatmenteffectonthismeasure.

TheeffectsizesinTable4areslightlylargerthansimilareffectsizesobtainedinevaluationsofthesamematerials conductedintheUnitedStates.Arecentevaluationwith76highschoolsandover16,000studentsfoundaneffectonself- reportedmindsetequalto34percentofastandarddeviationandaneffectonchallenge-seekingof23percentofastandard deviation(Yeageretal.,2016).Anevaluationin11highschools with3276studentsshowedaneffectonself-reported mindsetof30percentofastandarddeviation(Pauneskuetal.,2015).Thepresenteffectsseemtoexceedtheeffectsizesin theUnitedStates,perhapsbecausestudentspaidgreaterattentiontothematerialsintheNorwegiancontext.

InTable5,weinvestigatehowtreatmentaffectedeffortinthealgebraquestionsinSession3.Sincesomestudentsdid nothavetimetofinishallthequestions,or“gaveup”−clickingfastthroughthequestions,9welookathowmanycorrect answersstudentshadonthefirst10,20andall34questions.Themeasuresarestandardizedwithmeanzeroandstandard deviationoneusingtheSession3sample.InColumn1,weseenosignificantdifferenceineffortonthefirst10questions.

However,whencontrollingforbaselinevariablesinColumn2,wecanseethattreatedstudentshavesignificantlymore correctanswersonthefirst10questionscomparedtostudentsinthecontrolgroup.ConsistentwithHypothesis1,the estimatesuggeststhattreatedstudentsscored19percentofastandarddeviationhigherthancontrolstudents.Therewere nosignificantdifferencesbetweentreatedandcontrolwheninvestigatingtreatmenteffectsonthefirst20orall34questions inColumns3–6,suggestingnostrongtreatmenteffectontheoverallsample.

InTable6weinvestigatetreatmenteffectsfordifferentsubsamples.First,wecharacterizethestudentstohaveeithera fixedoragrowthpre-interventionmindsetbysplittingthesampleatthemeanofourmindsetmeasure.Consistentwith Hypotheses2,PanelsAandBdemonstratethatthetreatmenteffectdetectedinColumn2ofTable5isentirelydrivenby studentswhoinitiallyhadafixedmindset.Forthesestudents,thetreatmenteffectislargeandsignificantforallthree outcomemeasures.Theestimatessuggestthattreatedstudentsscored35,34and29percentofastandarddeviationhigher thancontrolstudentsonthefirst10,20andall34questions,respectively.Forstudentswhoinitiallyhadagrowthmindset, thereisnosignificanttreatmenteffect;theestimatedcoefficientisevennegativeinallbutonecolumn.

IntheremainingpanelsofTable6weinvestigateifreadilyobservablevariablesfromregistrydatacanhelpusidentify studentsparticularlyresponsivetotreatment.RecallthatTable3demonstratedthat,priortotreatment,afixedmindsetis

9 Eighteenstudents(7percent)didnotfinishallquestions.Amongthe236studentswhofinishedallquestions,theaveragetimeperquestiondeclined.

Thefirst13questionstookjustunder50sperquestion;questions14–20tookonaverageabout40sperquestion;questions21–27tookabout30sper question,andstudentsspentjustover20sperquestionontheremainingquestions.

(11)

Table6

TreatmentEffectonEffortinSession3.Subsampleanalyses.

ScoreonFirst10Questions ScoreonFirst20Questions ScoreonAll34Questions

(1) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6)

PanelA:Pre-TreatmentFixedMindset(n=130)

Treatment 0.297+ 0.348* 0.299+ 0.335** 0.245 0.285*

(0.178) (0.135) (0.170) (0.117) (0.170) (0.116)

AdjR-squared 0.014 0.434 0.016 0.538 0.008 0.540

PanelB:Pre-TreatmentGrowthMindset(n=124)

Treatment −0.094 0.022 −0.227 −0.059 −0.266 −0.129

(0.165) (0.114) (0.172) (0.119) (0.169) (0.121)

AdjR-squared 0.005 0.553 0.006 0.563 0.012 0.530

PanelC:VocationalTrack(n=133)

Treatment 0.234+ 0.272* 0.217+ 0.253* 0.222+ 0.251*

(0.133) (0.128) (0.117) (0.109) (0.119) (0.114)

AdjR-squared 0.016 0.131 0.018 0.189 0.018 0.143

PanelD:AcademicTrack(n=121)

Treatment 0.172 0.088 0.056 −0.029 −0.043 −0.110

(0.162) (0.122) (0.178) (0.116) (0.173) (0.121)

AdjR-squared 0.001 0.449 0.008 0.584 0.008 0.524

PanelE:LowGPA(n=129)

Treatment 0.180 0.286* 0.156 .226* 0.121 0.191+

(0.135) (0.128) (0.115) (0.108) (0.114) (0.106)

AdjR-squared 0.006 0.146 0.007 0.165 0.001 0.171

PanelF:HighGPA(n=125)

Treatment 0.148 0.074 0.053 −0.030 −0.006 −0.078

(0.165) (0.125) (0.169) (0.123) (0.174) (0.131)

AdjR-squared 0.002 0.458 0.007 0.496 0.008 0.459

Controlvariablesincluded No Yes No Yes No Yes

Notes:+p<0.10,*p<0.05,**p<0.01.Firstrowliststhedependentvariable.Eachpanelrepresentsadifferentsample.Forgivensample,eachrowpresentsa separateregressionandreportstheestimatedtreatmentcoefficient(robuststandarderror)andadjustedR-square.Lastrowsindicatewhethertheregression includesthecontrolvariables.ThecontrolvariablesareGPA,mathgrade,vocationaltrack(indicator),female(indicator),olderthan16(indicator),and questionassignmentorder(twoindicators).

particularlyprevalentamongstudentswithlowGPAandstudentsinthevocationaltrack.Thisisalsoclearlycommunicated inthetabulationsinTablesA1andA2intheAppendixA.Thus,weinvestigatetreatmenteffectssplittingthesamplebased ontrackandGPA(atthemedian).10

InPanelC,wecanseethatamongstudentsinthevocationaltrackthereisalargeandsignificanttreatmenteffect.Looking atthefirst10,20andall34questions,thetreatmenteffectis27,25and25percentofastandarddeviation,respectively.

Studentsintheacademictrackscoredhigherthanotherstudents,andasweshowinPanelD,thereisnosignificanttreatment effectforthem.

InPanelEweinvestigatetreatmenteffectsforstudentswithalowpre-treatmentGPA.Weseethatthereisasignificant treatmenteffectonallthreeoutcomemeasures.PanelFdemonstratesthatthereisnosignificanttreatmenteffectonthe studentswithahighpre-treatmentGPA.

SinceourmostinterestingresultsareinthevocationaltrackandlowGPAsamples,AppendixATablesA3–A5providethe balancetests,inadditiontoeffectestimatesonSession2outcomes,forthesesubsamples.FromthebalancetestsinTables A3andA4wefindthatbothsubsamplesarewellbalancedacrosstreatmentstatusonallcharacteristics,withtheexception ofsignificantdifferencesinFemalesandOlderthan16inthevocationaltracksample,andinFemaleandVocationaltrackin thelowGPAsample.Hence,allourreportedestimatesarecontrolledforallobservables.TableA5demonstratesthatalsoin thesesubsamplestherearelargeandsignificanteffectsonthemeasuresofgrowthmindsetandchallengeseekinginSession 3,andthemagnitudesaresimilartothemagnitudesestimatedforthefullsample.

10ThecorrelationbetweenhavingahighGPAandchoosingtheacademictrackinhighschoolis0.56.Evenifthiscorrelationishigh,theoverlapisbyno meansperfect,andconsequentlyinformativetosplitthesamplebothbasedontrackandGPA.

Referanser

RELATERTE DOKUMENTER

Organized criminal networks operating in the fi sheries sector engage in illicit activities ranging from criminal fi shing to tax crimes, money laundering, cor- ruption,

Recommendation 1 – Efficiency/sustainability: FishNET has been implemented cost-efficiently to some extent, and therefore not all funds will be spent before the project’s

However, this guide strongly recommends that countries still undertake a full corruption risk assessment, starting with the analysis discussed in sections 2.1 (Understanding

15 In the temperate language of the UN mission in Afghanistan (UNAMA), the operations of NDS Special Forces, like those of the Khost Protection Force, “appear to be coordinated

cessfully evacuated from the hospital and then transported all alive on British ships, escaping from a town which was under constant bombing and set on fire in the dramatic last

Within the scope of the medical movement, the Permanent Medical Commision of the Ministry of Health in 1851, based on a hypothesis that leprosy was a hereditary disease, proposed

From the above review of protection initiatives, three recurring issues can be discerned as particularly relevant for military contributions to protection activities: (i) the need

We presented the results of two separate measurement campaigns of the ultra wideband implant channel on living porcine subjects. To the best of our knowledge, this