• No results found

Regardless of the ease with which a decision is made, when aiming to nudge a consumer in a particular direction, what matters most is the final choice.

According to the results, the fifth hypothesis is confirmed in that the higher the nutritional distance between the two items of a pair, the more likely the revisited item is likely to be chosen.

This result appears unsurprising when reflecting on the fundamental reason why upgraded items are created. Assuming that a large part of these food decisions is taken based on health benefits, the worth of an upgrade is literally the value of its nutritional distance to its classic counterpart.

Admittedly, even in the case of lower distance pairs, participants were still more likely to choose revisited items than classic ones, however, any institution which would wish to maximize the popularity of an upgraded item on the market would do well to ensure that it differs from its regular version by an unambiguous margin.

Still, that correlation tend to suggest the existence of a reluctance toward upgraded items among some participants that becomes balanced and overcome as the worth of the upgrade grows.

Conclusion

After several decades of creation of upgraded food items by the processed food industry, starting from the low-fat products of the ‘80s to the more recent gluten-free products, the trend does not seem likely to end soon. Understanding how decisions regarding this specific class of items are made therefore seems like a promising topic.

As previously studied by the existing literature (Schulte-Mecklenbeck, 2013; Gigerenzer, 2011), this study confirms the prevalence of decision strategies making use of limited searches and of unequally weighted attributes.

However, in the context of upgraded food items, more likely to be found and purchased in the aisles of grocery stores, the prevalence of limited searches and of between-option searches shows the inadequacy between the way nutritional labels are displayed and the way information regarding nutritional attributes is processed. While all nutritional information is available to consumers, the placement and numerical nature of nutritional labels make difficult the assessment of their preferred product by consumers, whatever the attributes they value the most.

While it appears unsurprising that an item being revisited with better values in its macronutrients and calories attributes is more likely to be selected along those criteria – particularly so in the case of fats – it is interesting to note that participants seem to prefer the ingredient composition of classic items (often including components such as sugar, butter or eggs) to that of revisited items (generally containing less components) illustrating the importance of non-nutritional aspects of a food item.

The result that the greater an upgrade is – quantitatively speaking – the more likely it is to be chosen, and the more rapidly it is to be, can also appear intuitive. Nevertheless, the apparent differences in

handling of food item pairs depending on their nutritional difference suggest the potential of future research in food item upgrades and in consumers’ appreciation of the value of these upgrades.

The difference in weighting of the fat attribute compared to other macronutrients for example suggests that a nutritional distance weighting equally all attributes might not be the best metric to translate in a quantitative way the perception of the value brought to a consumer by an upgrade.

Finally, the qualitative aspects of an upgrade are probably equally worthy of interest. Given the suggestion that people perceive differently savory and sweet items or fat-rich and carbohydrate-rich items (Green, 1996), the threshold levels of values brought by an upgrade which necessary to appeal to a consumer are likely to depend on the type of items involved as well as the nutritional

dimensions which are upgraded.

References

Alter, A. L., Oppenheimer, D. M., Epley, N., & Eyre, R. N. (2007). Overcoming intuition:

metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(4), 569.

Bender, M. M., & Derby, B. M. (1992). Prevalence of reading nutrition and ingredient information on food labels among adult Americans: 1982–1988. Journal of Nutrition Education, 24(6), 292-297.

Böckenholt, U., & Hynan, L. S. (1994). Caveats on a process-tracing measure and a remedy. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 7(2), 103–117.

Cockburn, A. (1977). DIETARY GOALS FOR UNITED-STATES-STAFF OF SELECT-COMMITTEE-ON-NUTRITION-AND-HUMAN-NEEDS, UNITED-STATES-SENATE.

Connors, M., Bisogni, C. A., Sobal, J., & Devine, C. M. (2001). Managing values in personal food systems. Appetite, 36, 189–200.

Duchowski, A. (2007). Eye tracking methodology: Theory and practice (Vol. 373). Springer Science &

Business Media.

Evans, J. S. B. (2003). In two minds: dual-process accounts of reasoning. Trends in cognitive sciences, 7(10), 454-459.

Feenberg, D., Ganguli, I., Gaule, P., & Gruber, J. (2017). It’s good to be first: Order bias in reading

and citing NBER working papers. Review of Economics and Statistics, 99(1), 32-39.

Fitts, P. M. (1954). The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude of movement. Journal of experimental psychology, 47(6), 381.

Ford, J. K., Schmitt, N., Schechtmann, S. L., Hults, B. M., & Doherty, M. L. (1989). Process tracing methods: Contributions, problems and neglected research questions. Organizational Behavior &

Human Decision Processes, 43, 75-117.

Gigerenzer, G., Hertwig, H., & Pachur, T. (2011). Introduction. In G. Gigerenzer, R. Hertwig, & T.

Pachur (Eds.), Heuristics. The foundations of adaptive behavior (pp. 17–23). New York, NY:

Oxford University Press.

Goldberg, J.H., Probart, C.K., Zak, R.E., (1999). Visual search of food nutrition labels. Hum.

Factors 41, 425–437.

Graham, D.J., Jeffery, R.W., (2011). Location, location, location: eye tracking evidence that consumers preferentially view prominently positioned nutrition information. J. Am. Diet. Assoc.

111, 1704–1711

Graham, D. J., Lucas-Thompson, R. G., Mueller, M. P., Jaeb, M., & Harnack, L. (2017). Impact of explained v. unexplained front-of-package nutrition labels on parent and child food choices: a randomized trial. Public Health Nutrition, 20(5), 774-785.

Green, S. M., & Blundell, J. E. (1996). Subjective and objective indices of the satiating effect of foods. Can people predict how filling a food will be?. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 50(12), 798-806.

Grunert, K. G., Wills, J. M., & Fernández-Celemín, L. (2010). Nutrition knowledge, and use and understanding of nutrition information on food labels among consumers in the UK. Appetite, 55(2), 177-189.

Ha, R. Y., Namkoong, K., Kang, J. I., Kim, Y. T., & Kim, S. J. (2009). Interaction between serotonin transporter promoter and dopamine receptor D4 polymorphisms on decision making. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 33(7), 1217-1222.

Higgins, S., & Semper, H. (2016). The effects of nutrition label format on healthier dietary choices: a forced choice eye-tracking study. European Health Psychologist, 18(S), 559.

Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1976). Eye fixations and cognitive processes. Cognitive Psychology, 8, 441-480.

Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Macmillan.

Landauer, T. K.; Nachbar, D. W. (1985). "Selection from alphabetic and numeric menu trees using a touch screen". Proceedings of the SIGCHI conference on Human factors in computing systems - CHI '85. p. 73

Leonard, T. C. (2008). Richard H. Thaler, Cass R. Sunstein, Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. Constitutional Political Economy, 19(4), 356-360.

Marois, R., & Ivanoff, J. (2005). Capacity limits of information processing in the brain. Trends in cognitive sciences, 9(6), 296-305.

Martin, R. (2001). "Koala". In Macdonald, D. Encyclopedia of Mammals (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press. pp. 852–54.

Martin, R. D. (1983). Human brain evolution in an ecological context. New York: American Museum of natural history.

Martinez, S. (2017, April 5). New Products. Retrieved May 5, 2017. from http://www.ers.usda.gov

Nederkoorn, C., Guerrieri, R., Havermans, R. C., Roefs, A., & Jansen, A. (2009). The interactive effect of hunger and impulsivity on food intake and purchase in a virtual supermarket. International journal of obesity, 33(8), 905-912.

Neuhouser, M. L., Kristal, A. R., & Patterson, R. E. (1999). Use of food nutrition labels is associated with lower fat intake. Journal of the American dietetic Association, 99(1), 45-53.

Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., Coupey, E., & Johnson, E. J. (1992). A constructive process view of decision making: Multiple strategies in judgment and choice. Acta Psychologica, 80, 107-141.

Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R., & Johnson, E. J. (1993). The adaptive decision maker. Cambridge:

Cambridge University Press.

Pollan, M. (2006). The omnivore's dilemma: A natural history of four meals. Penguin.

Riedl, R., Brandstätter, E., & Roithmayr, F. (2008). Identifying decision strategies: A process-and outcome-based classification method. Behavior Research Methods, 40(3), 795-807.

Roosevelt, T. quoted by John M. Kost (25 July 1995) S. 946, the Information Technology Management Reform Act of 1995: hearing before the Subcommittee on Oversight of Government Management and the District of Columbia of the Committee on Governmental Affairs (1996).

Roussos, P., Giakoumaki, S. G., & Bitsios, P. (2009). Cognitive and emotional processing in high novelty seeking associated with the L-DRD4 genotype. Neuropsychologia, 47(7), 1654-1659.

Rozin, P. (1976). The selection of foods by rats, humans, and other animals. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 6, 21-76.

Savage LJ. (1954). The Foundations of Statistics. New York: Dover. 2nd ed.

Scheibehenne, B., Miesler, L., & Todd, P. M. (2007). Fast and frugal food choices. Uncovering individual decision heuristics. Appetite, 49(3), 578–589.

Schulte-Mecklenbeck, M., Sohn, M., de Bellis, E., Martin, N., & Hertwig, R. (2013). A lack of appetite for information and computation. Simple heuristics in food choice. Appetite, 71, 242-251.

Shah AK, Oppenheimer DM. (2008). Heuristics made easy: an effort-reduction framework. Psychol.

Bull. 137:207–22

Simon HA. (1979). Rational decision making in business organizations. Am. Econ. Rev. 69:493–513 Tversky A, Kahneman D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Science

185:1124–30

Variyam, J. N. (2008). Do nutrition labels improve dietary outcomes? Health economics, 17(6), 695-708.

Wansink, B., & Sobal, J. (2007). Mindless eating the 200 daily food decisions we overlook.

Environment and Behavior, 39(1), 106-123.

Willemsen, M. C., & Johnson, E. J. (2009). MouselabWEB: Monitoring information acquisition processes on the Web.

Wansink, B. (2007). Mindless eating. Why we eat more than we think. New York, NY: Bantam.

Wansink, B., Painter, J. E., & North, J. (2005). Bottomless bowls. Why visual cues of portion size may influence intake. Obesity Research, 13(1), 93–100.

Wright, P. (1974). The harassed decision maker: Time pressures, distractions, and the use of evidence. Faculty working papers; no. 0134.

Zagorin, R. K. (1977). Sociology of food. IDRC, Ottawa, ON, CA.

Zeithaml, V. A. (1988). Consumer perceptions of price, quality, and value: a means-end model and synthesis of evidence. The Journal of marketing, 2-22.

Revisited Classic Revisited Classic Revisited Classic Revisited Classic Revisited Classic Revisited Classic

Calories 180 209 50 278 350 547 270 358 15 110 32 144

Carbohydrates 15 16 12 69 80 50 22 53 4 6 8 30

Fats 12 15 0 0 0 38 18 15 0 8 0 2

Proteins 2 4 1 0 0 7 3 5 0 3 1 1

Strawberry sherbet

Chocolate mousse Orange marmelade Chips Chocolate cake Tzatziki

Appendix

Appendix A: Food items attributes

Appendix B: One-sample t-test of basic metrics

One-Sample Test Test Value = 0

t df Sig. (2-tailed) Mean Difference 95% Confidence Interval of the Difference

Lower Upper

Completeness 23.053 719 .000 .425 .39 .46

SearchMetric 20.220 719 .000 .363 .33 .40

EqualWeight 33.321 719 .000 .607 .57 .64

Appendix C: Chi-square of basic metrics by choice

Appendix D: Chi-square of favorite weighted attribute by choice

Choice Total

Appendix E: ANOVA of decision time by nutritional distance group

Descriptives

TotalTime

N Mean Std. Deviation Std. Error 95% Confidence Interval for Mean Minimum Maximum Lower Bound Upper Bound

Low Distance 117 20914.75 10682.592 987.606 18958.67 22870.83 2793 74335

Medium Distance 362 11141.42 7196.354 378.232 10397.60 11885.23 535 44859

High Distance 241 13681.66 8536.417 549.879 12598.46 14764.87 557 70261

Total 720 13579.86 8971.557 334.350 12923.44 14236.28 535 74335

Appendix F: ANOVA of nutritional distance by choice

Descriptives NutriDistance

N Mean Std. Deviation Std. Error 95% Confidence Interval for Mean Minimum Maximum Lower Bound Upper Bound

Classic 244 108.1038 61.56371 3.94121 100.3405 115.8671 29.24 235.02

Revisited 476 139.4914 71.33358 3.26957 133.0668 145.9160 29.24 235.02

Total 720 128.8545 69.73964 2.59904 123.7519 133.9571 29.24 235.02

Appendix G: Chi-square of nutritional distance group by choice

NutriGroup * Choice Crosstabulation

RELATERTE DOKUMENTER