• No results found

Allomorphy: Old Concept, Big Data, New Model

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Share "Allomorphy: Old Concept, Big Data, New Model"

Copied!
1
0
0

Laster.... (Se fulltekst nå)

Fulltekst

(1)

Allomorphy: Old Concept, Big Data, New Model

New methods of analysis for rival polysemous affixes

Author: Dr. Anna Endresen University of Tromsø: The Arctic University of Norway Department of Language and Linguistics CLEAR research group

THIS STUDY IN A NUTSHELL

I revisit an old concept of Allomorphy, which was first introduced to linguistics in the 1940s by American Structuralists. Despite fruitful

discussions, the most rigid approach (Harris 1942) to Allomorphy persisted in the history of the field.  

 

I challenge this notion with data on 15 Russian

prefixes (4,718 lexemes collected from the corpus and in 2 experiments with 60 and 120 subjects).

I find that the conventional understanding of Allomorphy is a theoretical construct, an

idealization. It fails to capture properties of data.  

I propose an alternative model of Allomorphy.

It is more accurate and realistic with regard to such properties of data as gradience,

semantic dissimilation of allomorphs, and overlap in their distribution.  

Email: anna.endresen@uit.no

Web page: h"ps://sites.google.com/site/annaendresen/  

You can find the details in the thesis at h"p://hdl.handle.net/10037/7098  

Data  and  R  scripts  are  available  at  TROLLing  at  h"p://hdl.handle.net/10037.1/10078  

ABSTRACT Many linguistic concepts were first introduced in the Structuralist Era, the time when linguists believed in clear-cut oppositions and did not have access to large corpora. I find that allomorphy is a scalar phenomenon that can be best captured in terms of a radial category.

The new model is based on quantitative methods and can handle semantic dissimilation of allomorphs as well as distributional overlap. I show how statistical models turn allomorphy into a measurable and verifiable correspondence of form and meaning.

CONCEPT WITH STRUCTURALIST BAGGAGE (+/-)

Zellig Harris (1942): “We can arrange morpheme alternants into units in exactly the same manner as we arrange sound types into phonemes.” è

•  “A morpheme unit is a group of alternants which have the same meaning and

complementary distribution.” MOST RIGID MODEL

Old  Concept   New  Model  

Charles Hockett (1947): the analogy “(allo)phone : phoneme = morph : morpheme”

•  Amendment: Non-contrastive distribution: i) complementary distribution or ii) partial complementation, i.e. free variation in the environments where both

alternants can occur” (e.g. you and me vs. you and I). LESS RIGID MODEL Eugene Nida (1948): Morphemes are meaningful units, different from phonemes.

•  Amendment: No items that are different in form are absolutely identical in meaning. è“From the difference in their distribution they acquire a certain

difference in meaning.” FLEXIBLE MODEL

Coined the term ALLOMORPH

We can elaborate this flexible and non-absolute understanding of allomorphy and enrich it with advances of computational models, psycholinguistic experiments, and corpus data.

Big  Data  

Prefixes   Number   of   analyzed  

verbs  

Formal  

similarity   Etymolo-­‐

gical   relaCon-­‐

ship  

SemanCcs   DistribuCon    

Status  

#  of  shared  

submeanings   Shared  prototype   DisCnct  profiles   Size  of  

overlap   CondiConing  

factors   RAZ-­‐  

RAS-­‐   200   similar   related   share  all  7  

submeanings     share  ‘APART’   No   no  overlap   phonology   Prototypical  

allomorphy   RAZ-­‐  

RAZO-­‐   210   similar   related   share  all  7  

submeanings     share  ‘APART’   No   no  overlap  

  phonology  &  

morphophonology   Standard  

allomorphy   S-­‐  

SO-­‐   1,156   similar   related   share  all  6  

submeanings   share  both  ‘DOWNWARD’  

&  ‘CENTRIPETAL’   Yes:  

in  ‘CONCOMITANT  

ACTION’  

15  minimal  

pairs   phonology,  

morphophonology,   register,  semanUcs  

Non-­‐Standard   allomorphy   O-­‐  

OB-­‐  

OBO-­‐  

1,037   similar   related   share  all  15  

submeanings     share  ‘AROUND’  

  Yes:  

spaUal  vs.  change-­‐

of-­‐state  

23  minimal  

pairs   phonology,  

semanUcs  (type  of   base),  prosody  

Non-­‐Standard   allomorphy   PERE-­‐  

PRE-­‐   945   similar   related   share  8  out  of  

14  submeanings     share  ‘TRANSFER  OVER/

ACROSS’   Yes:  

spaUal  vs.  intensity   22  minimal  

pairs   grammaUcal  

classes:  verbs  vs.  

non-­‐verbs  

Non-­‐Standard   allomorphy   VZ-­‐  

VOZ-­‐   384   similar   related   share  all  9  

submeanings   share  ‘UPWARD’  but  

differ  in  height   Yes:  spaUal,   metaphorical,  

aspectual  

21  minimal  

pairs   semanUcs,  register,  

akUonsart   Non-­‐Standard  

allomorphy   (borderline  case)   VY-­‐  

IZ-­‐   998   not  

similar   different  

sources   share  10  out  of  

12  submeanings   share  ‘OUT  OF’,  but  do  

not  share  share  ‘ZIGZAG’   Yes:  

‘OUT  OF’  vs.  ‘EXHAUST’   112  minimal  

pairs   semanUcs,  register  

(prosody)   Non-­‐Standard   allomorphy   (borderline  case)   O-­‐  

U-­‐   155   not  

similar   unrelated   share  the   submeaning  

‘make  X  be  Y’  

different  prototypes  

‘AROUND’  and  ‘AWAY’   Not  applicable   17  minimal  

pairs   qualitaUve  vs.  

relaUonal  adjecUval   base  

Non-­‐Allomorphy;  

Closely  associated   rival  morphemes   PRE-­‐  

PRI-­‐  

PRED-­‐  

10   similar   unrelated   no  shared  

submeanings   different  prototypes   Not  applicable   some  

overlap   different  semanUcs   Non-­‐Allomorphy;  

Different  morphemes   with  no  associaUon  

Prototypical

Standard Standard Non-Standard

Non-Standard

NEW MODEL: ALLOMORPHY AS A RADIAL CATEGORY

Non-Allomorphy

Non-Allomorphy

Allomorphy  is  broader  than  its   convenUonal  understanding.  

Allomorphy  is  a  scalar   relaConship  between   morpheme  variants  –  a   relaUonship  that  can  vary  in  

terms  of  closeness  and   regularity.  

The  core  clear  cases  of   allomorphy  can  be  viewed  as  

prototypical  rather  than  the   only  possible.  

Prototypical  Allomorphy  is  characterized   by  the  closest  and  most  automaUc  

associaUon  of  formants.  Typically  

phonologically  condiUoned  by  a  regular,   automaUc,  and  producUve  phonological  

rule.  E.g.:  Russian  prefixes  RAZ-­‐/RAS-­‐  

Standard  Allomorphy  –  saUsfies  both   criteria  (idenUcal  meaning  &  

complementary  distribuUon),  but  is   governed  by  factors  other  than  (or  in  

addiUon  to)  acUve  phonology  – morphophonology,  register,  

semanUcs.  E.g.:  prefixes  RAZ-­‐/RAZO-­‐    

Non-­‐Standard  Allomorphy  –  violates  one   or  both  criteria  BUT  shows  a  strong   semanUc  similarity  or  robust  pa"ern  of  

distribuUon.    

E.g.:  Russian  prefixes  O-­‐/OB-­‐,  S-­‐/SO-­‐,   PERE-­‐/PRE-­‐,  VZ-­‐/VOZ-­‐,  VY-­‐/IZ-­‐  

Allomorphy  is  a  gradient   phenomenon  –  with  a  central   prototype,  standard  exemplars  

and  non-­‐standard  deviaUons.  

AlternaCve  to  the  

all-­‐or-­‐nothing  model:   New  disCncCons:   CASE STUDY OF THE PROTOTYPE:

The Russian prefixes RAZ- / RAS- ‘A PART

CASE STUDY OF NON-STANDARD ALLOMORPHY:

The Russian prefixes O- / OB- ‘A ROUND

CASE STUDY OF NON-STANDARD ALLOMORPHY:

The Russian prefixes VZ- / VOZ- ‘U PWARD

DeviaCons  are  recognized  as   Allomorphy  or  Non-­‐Allomorphy  

on  the  basis  of  staUsUcal   measurements.  

This results from the process of semantic dissimilation of former phonological variants.

The result of interaction and co-evolution of the native

Russian prefix VZ- and a cognate loan prefix VOZ-.  

SonorityOnset p < 0.001

1

voiceless {sonorant, voiced, vowel}

Node 2 (n = 102)

razras

0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8

1 Node 3 (n = 98)

razras

0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1

SonorityOnset SimpleOrClusterOnset Metaphor PerfType Semantics

0.00.10.20.30.40.5

Modeling  of  prefix  polysemy:  200  verbs  

DistribuUon  of  RAZ-­‐  and  RAS-­‐  across  verbs   and  prefix  submeanings  is  not  significantly   different:  p  =  0.46  

Radial  category  profiling:  

The  choice  of  RAZ-­‐  vs.  RAS-­‐  is   phonologically  condiUoned  by  a   producUve  and  excepUonless   process  of  regressive  voicing   assimilaUon:  

Sonority  of  the  onset  base  

(voiced  vs.  voiceless  consonant)   is  the  only  predictor  of  the  

prefix:  

The  diagram  shows  how  many  verbs  is  a"ested  for   each  submeaning  of  the  prefix.  

è  SemanCcs  plays  no  role  in  the  distribuCon  of  RAZ-­‐  and  RAS-­‐.  

SEMANTICS:  Highly  polysemous  prefixes   è  How  do   we  assess  whether  they  are  idenUcal  in  meaning?  

   

Data:  1,037  verbs  prefixed  in  O-­‐  and  OB-­‐  

Single  radial  network  of  15  submeanings  

Radial  category  profiling:  

Different  profiles  of  O-­‐  and  OB-­‐  in   terms  of  type  frequency  of  verbs   a"ested  for  each  submeaning:  

0%  

10%  

20%  

30%  

40%  

50%  

60%  

70%  

Move  around;   Pass  by;  Affect  a   Deceive;  Overdo   Envelop;   Metaphorical   Surround   Metaphorical   surround   Affect  a  surface   Impose  /  acquire  a   new  quality   Mistake   O-­‐  %   OB-­‐  %  

DISTRIBUTION:  governed  by  several  factors  

(phonological,  semanUc,  prosodic)   è  How  do  we   determine  which  factor  is  the  most  powerful?  

   

Manner p < 0.001 1

{affricate, fricative, stop} sonorant

StimulusType p < 0.001

2

verb adjective

ClusterOnset p < 0.001

3

no yes

Node 4 (n = 889)

O OBOBO 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1

Node 5 (n = 251)

OOBOBO 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1

PossibleWithB p < 0.001

6

no yes

Node 7 (n = 126)

OOBOBO 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1

ClusterOnset p = 0.001

8

no yes

Node 9 (n = 880)

OOBOBO 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1

Node 10 (n = 126)

O OBOBO 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1

StimulusType p = 0.042

11

adjective verb

Place p = 0.017

12

labial{alveopalatal, dental}

Node 13 (n = 65)

O OBOBO 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1

Node 14 (n = 252)

O OBOBO 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1

Node 15 (n = 315)

OOBOBO 0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1

ClassificaUon   tree  model:  

Experimental   data    

Manner StimulusType Place ClusterOnset PossibleWithB 0.000.020.040.06

Random  Forests  

model:  

Importance  scores  

The  phonological  factor  is  stronger   than  the  semanCc  factor.  

The  13 th  InternaUonal  CogniUve  LinguisUcs  Conference  

20-­‐25  July  2015,  Northumbria  University,  Newcastle  upon  Tyne,  UK  

Data:  384  verbs  prefixed  in   VZ-­‐  and  VOZ-­‐  

Different  radial  category  profiles  

0%  

5%  

10%  

15%  

20%  

25%  

30%  

35%  

1.MOVE  

UPWARD   2.VIOLATE  A  

SURFACE   3.AGITATE  

EMOTION.   4.RESIST   5.HIGH  

DOMINANT  

STATUS  

6.BACK   7.GROW  UP   8.INGRESS.   9.SEMELF.   Standard  verbs  in  VZ-­‐   Standard  verbs  in  VOZ-­‐  

Unique  situaCon  in  Slavic:  the  naUve   prefix  VZ-­‐  and  the  loan  prefix  VOZ-­‐  

have  been  coexisUng  in  Russian  since   their  formal  differenUaUon  emerged   in  the  14

th

 c.    

AkUonsarten   Metaphorical  

submeanings   SpaUal  

  The  high  alUtude  of   VOZ-­‐  moUvates  gradual   entry  into  a  new  state   of  affairs  (Ingressive).  

  The  short  trajectory  of   VZ-­‐  jusUfies  abruptness   of  a  rapid  momentary   event  (SemelfacUve).  

   

Indo-­‐European  *ups   Proto-­‐Slavic  *vъz   Church  Slavonic    

(South  Slavic)   voz-­‐  

Russian   (East  Slavic)  

vz-­‐  

In  Modern  Russian  VZ-­‐  and  VOZ-­‐  

encode  different  height  (vozvysit’sja  

‘tower’  vs.  vsplyt’  ‘rise  to  the  surface’)  

è

è

Single  radial  network  of  9  submeanings  

Referanser

RELATERTE DOKUMENTER

Norges Bank has developed a new quantitative model for analysing banks’ credit risk in the enterprise sector. The new model predicts individual bankruptcy probabilities as a function

The pur- pose of this chapter is to present “the aesthetic model of disability”, a new model that can be used as a supplement to the recognized social and relational models

By doing so, we introduce the new concept of “site risk” and a new risk assessment model, called Construction Site Risk Assessment Tool (CONSRAT), that takes the site as a unit

These allow us to solve a family of recurrences (Theorem 1.35) that appear in a natural way in the computation of moments of balance indices under the Uniform model for bifur-

When the focus ceases to be comprehensive health care to the whole population living within an area and becomes instead risk allocation to individuals, members, enrollees or

If on the other hand, the boundary segment is a line or a curve, the wall repulsion acting on agent a from boundary segment b should ideally equal the total crowd repulsion force on

In the present case, UDFs are used both for extracting information from the turbulent velocity field for input to the model and for calculating the evaporation rate; the

Building on insights from media and technology studies that new media should be understood for how they overlap with old media, the article reports an ethnographic study of