• No results found

Social environment shapes female settlement decisions in a solitary carnivore

N/A
N/A
Protected

Academic year: 2022

Share "Social environment shapes female settlement decisions in a solitary carnivore"

Copied!
10
0
0

Laster.... (Se fulltekst nå)

Fulltekst

(1)

Behavioral

Ecology ISBE

International Society for Behavioral Ecology Behavioral Ecology (2021), XX(XX), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/arab118

Address correspondence to J.E. Hansen. E-mail: [email protected].

© The Author(s) 2021. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Society for Behavioral Ecology.

This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted reuse, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Original Article

Social environment shapes female settlement decisions in a solitary carnivore

J.E. Hansen,

a,

A.G. Hertel,

a,b,

S.C. Frank,

a,

J. Kindberg,

c,d

and A. Zedrosser

a,e

a

Faculty of Technology, Natural Sciences and Maritime Sciences, Department of Natural Sciences and Environmental Health, University of South-Eastern Norway, Bø i Telemark, Norway,

b

Senkenberg Biodiversity and Climate Research Centre, Frankfurt, Germany,

c

Norwegian Institute for Nature Research, Trondheim, Norway,

d

Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, Department of Wildlife, Fish, and Environmental Studies, Umeå, Sweden, and

e

Department of Integrative Biology, Institute of Wildlife Biology and Game Management, University of Natural Resources and Applied Life Sciences, Vienna, Austria

Received 28 October 2020; revised 25 August 2021; editorial decision 27 September 2021; accepted 1 October 2021.

How and where a female selects an area to settle and breed is of central importance in dispersal and population ecology as it governs range expansion and gene flow. Social structure and organization have been shown to influence settlement decisions, but its impor- tance in the settlement of large, solitary mammals is largely unknown. We investigate how the identity of overlapping conspecifics on the landscape, acquired during the maternal care period, influences the selection of settlement home ranges in a non-territorial, solitary mammal using location data of 56 female brown bears (Ursus arctos). We used a resource selection function to determine whether females’ settlement behavior was influenced by the presence of their mother, related females, familiar females, and female population density. Hunting may remove mothers and result in socio-spatial changes before settlement. We compared overlap be- tween settling females and their mother’s concurrent or most recent home ranges to examine the settling female’s response to the absence or presence of her mother on the landscape. We found that females selected settlement home ranges that overlapped their mother’s home range, familiar females, that is, those they had previously overlapped with, and areas with higher density than their natal ranges. However, they did not select areas overlapping related females. We also found that when mothers were removed from the landscape, female offspring selected settlement home ranges with greater overlap of their mother’s range, compared with mothers who were alive. Our results suggest that females are acquiring and using information about their social environment when making set- tlement decisions.

Key words: dispersal, public information, settlement, social environment, space use.

INTRODUCTION

How and where a female selects an area to settle and breed is of central importance in dispersal and population ecology (Pulliam and Danielson 1991; Barton 1992; Stamps 2001). After a variable amount of time living in a natal home range (NHR), selection of a settlement area is the final stage in natal dispersal, that is, dispersal before breeding (Bowler and Benton 2005). A common settlement pattern among mammals is for subadult males to disperse and for females to remain philopatric, that is, settle where they overlap their NHR (Waser and Jones 1983). However, even in mammals with general female philopatry it is common for some females to disperse (Lawson Handley and Perrin 2007), and such plasticity in

dispersal pattern suggests behavioral control over the settlement process (Benard and McCauley 2008).

Proximate cues in the natal period may influence where indi- vidual females settle (Stamps 2001; Benard and McCauley 2008).

Natal habitat preference induction is a mechanism whereby indi- viduals use environmental cues from their NHR when searching for settlement areas (Stamps and Davis 2006). Another mech- anism is density dependence (Matthysen 2005), that is, females living at higher population densities may be limited in areas available to settle and breed (Fretwell and Lucas 1969; Stockley and Bro-Jørgensen 2011). Kin-based dispersal, that is, females settle in areas away from related individuals to avoid kin compe- tition and thereby increase their inclusive fitness, has also been proposed as a mechanism for female settlement decisions (Cote and Clobert 2010). Conversely, in other social systems females settle where they overlap kin with increased tolerance towards

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/advance-article/doi/10.1093/beheco/arab118/6398953 by Norsk Institutt for Naturforskning, Library user on 22 November 2021

(2)

related individuals, which may also increase inclusive fitness (Clutton-Brock and Lukas 2012).

The influence of social information, that is, information ac- quired by observing other individuals interact with the environment (Danchin 2004), on the settlement process is receiving attention as a mechanism influencing settlement patterns (Vercken et al. 2012; Wey et  al. 2015). The use of social information for settlement decisions has been described for a variety of taxa (Doligez et al. 2002; Nocera et al. 2006; Robinson et al. 2011; Vercken et al. 2012), but has re- ceived less attention in mammals (Valone 2007). This implies that so- cial structure (relationships and interactions among individuals) and social organization (the composition of individuals within an area or group), that is, the social environment, can provide information for individual dispersal decisions (Bowler and Benton 2005; Armansin et  al. 2020). For example, conspecific density can be used to assess the quality of a resource or an environment (Dall et  al. 2005). In addition, information about the social environment gleaned in the natal period, for example, the presence and location of conspecifics on the landscape (Danchin et al. 2004) or the detection of a female with dependent young (Clobert et  al. 2001), can provide naïve in- dividuals with valuable information, for example, on habitat availa- bility or reproductive competition, and reduce uncertainty for their settlement decisions (Danchin et al. 2004). Lastly, the use of informa- tion regarding specific female identities allows an individual to gain familiarity with overlapping conspecifics which can reduce ‘social re- sistance’ into an area for settlement (Armansin et al. 2020).

Research on the influence of the social environment in mammals is typically conducted on social species (Hare et  al. 2014; O’Mara et  al. 2014). We use the brown bear (Ursus arctos) as model species to investigate if the social environment influences natal dispersal and the selection of settlement home ranges (SHR) in a non-territorial, solitary mammal. Brown bears live solitarily for the majority of their lives aside from the mating period and females rearing offspring.

Adult females maintain relatively stable home ranges with extensive spatio-temporal overlap, especially among related individuals (Mace and Waller 1997; Støen et al. 2005). Female brown bears likely ex- hibit inverse density dependent dispersal, which may result in the formation of matrilineal assemblages, that is, overlapping home ranges of several generations of related females (Støen et al. 2005).

Reproductive suppression has been documented in female brown bears, and it has been suggested that related females (Støen et  al.

2006b) and neighboring females (Ordiz et  al. 2008) influence one another’s breeding patterns. After the death of an adult female, other females will shift their home ranges to fill in that vacancy (Frank et al.

2017). This suggests that female brown bears may make decisions on space use and reproduction based on information regarding the so- cial environment. Information gathering from conspecifics may re- duce uncertainty in the settlement process and result in an increased chance of successful breeding (Danchin et al. 2001). Brown bears ex- tensively use chemical scent cues (Clapham et al. 2012, 2014; Jojola et al. 2012; Morehouse et al. 2021) and other spoor on the landscape (Sergiel et  al. 2017), and scent communication is the most likely means of acquiring information on conspecifics (Revilla et al. 2021).

Additionally, information about the social environment may be obtained through direct social interactions with spatially overlapping females. We propose that females acquire information about the so- cial environment during the natal period, such as the identity and density of conspecifics, that is later used in the selection of an SHR.

The primary objective of this study is to investigate if the social environment influences selection of an SHR by a solitary-living mammal, the brown bear. We hypothesized that, amongst all female

conspecifics, an individual female’s mother would exhibit the highest level of social tolerance towards them. We thus predicted that (P1) females would select SHR that overlapped their mother’s home range. However, hunting in the population leads to regular socio- spatial changes (Frank et  al. 2017), and a mother may die before a female’s settlement decision. We hypothesized that females detect the presence or absence of their mother on the landscape and would use that information when making settlement decisions. Specifically, we predicted that (P2) females whose mothers died before settlement would overlap a greater amount of their mother’s home range than females whose mothers were alive in the settlement period. A  kin- based socio-spatial structure has been documented in female brown bears (Støen et al. 2005) and we hypothesized that females recognize and show higher social tolerance to kin than non-kin. We thus pre- dicted that (P3) females would select SHR that overlapped related females. The presence of females who are familiar from the natal period (Mateo 2002) may decrease aggressive encounters between neighboring females and improve breeding success (Ylönen et  al.

1990; Armansin et al. 2020). We hypothesized that during the natal period females gained familiarity with neighboring females who may be less hostile to a female in the settlement period. We, therefore, predicted that (P4) female bears would select SHR that overlapped with females “known” from the natal period, hereafter “familiar” fe- males. Previous research on female brown bears indicated that den- sity patterns are regulated through social interactions of females and that individuals have the ability to detect local density levels and that reproductive success of primiparous females is higher in lower den- sity areas (Støen et  al. 2006a; Zedrosser et  al. 2009). We hypothe- sized that females detect density levels of conspecifics in the natal period and during the selection of an SHR. We, therefore, predicted (P5) females would select an SHR with a density lower relative to the NHR to reduce competition.

MATERIALS AND METHODS Study area and model species

Our study area covers ~13,000 km2 and is located in Gävleborg and Dalarna counties, southcentral Sweden (~61ºN, 14ºE). The terrain is hilly, and Scots pine (Pinus sylvestris) and Norway spruce (Picea abies) are the dominating tree species. The forests are inten- sively managed, resulting in a mosaic of mixed-aged stands, bogs, and lakes. The human population density in the region is low (Ordiz et al. 2012), but there exists an extensive network of forestry roads (Frank et al. 2015). Bears are hunted in the study area (Frank et al. 2017).

Brown bears are solitary-living and non-territorial carnivores with a promiscuous mating system and extensive overlap of male and female home ranges (Støen et  al. 2005; Steyaert et  al. 2012).

The mating season is from May to July (Steyaert et  al. 2012) and females give birth to cubs during hibernation in January (Friebe et al. 2001). Offspring in the Scandinavian population remain with their mothers in the NHR for either 1.5 or 2.5  years until family break up (Van de Walle et al. 2018). Dispersal is a gradual process over a 2–3 year timespan, and up to 54% of the female subadults will settle in areas overlapping their NHR (Støen et  al. 2006a).

The average age of female primiparity in this population is 5 years (Zedrosser et al. 2009).

Telemetry data and home range estimation

The population in the study area has been continuously moni- tored since 1985 (Swenson et al. 1994). Very-high frequency (VHF)

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/advance-article/doi/10.1093/beheco/arab118/6398953 by Norsk Institutt for Naturforskning, Library user on 22 November 2021

(3)

telemetry data span from 1985 to 2016 and Global Positioning System (GPS) telemetry data from 2003 to 2018 (Dahle and Swenson 2003; Arnemo and Evans 2017). Capture and handling of bears was conducted by permit under Swedish authorities and ethical committees (Uppsala Djurförsöksetiska Nämnd: C40/3, C212/9, C47/9, C210/10, C7/12, C268/12, C18/15. Statens Veterinärmediciniska Anstalt, Jordbruksverket, Naturvårdsverket:

Dnr 35-846/03, Dnr 412-7093-08 NV, Dnr 412-7327-09 Nv, Dnr 31-11102/12, NV-01758-14). Relocations from VHF collars were obtained weekly on average and GPS collar data were collected hourly. We inspected VHF locations for outliers and removed lo- cations that occurred beyond the average home range diameter (14 km) from the cluster of locations of a given individual. To mini- mize location error of GPS relocation data, we removed GPS loca- tions with a dilution of precision (DOP) > 10 from the data (D’eon and Delparte 2005). We then subset the screened telemetry data to the average local active period for bears, 01 May to 30 September (Friebe et al. 2001).

We generated annual 95% kernel utilization distributions (UD) using the R package adehabitatHR (Calenge 2006) for each focal bear. A focal bear’s NHR was estimated from the UD of its mother in the year before family breakup, as focal bears had not yet received radio collars during this time period. We estimated a focal bear’s SHR from its own annual UD two years after family breakup, before primiparity (Zedrosser et  al. 2009). We selected two years post family breakup because previous work has shown peak dispersal activity for females in the year after family break up (Støen et  al. 2006a) and our goal was to best estimate where fe- males settled. For some focal bears (n  =  24), telemetry data were unavailable for the second year after family breakup. In these cases, we used the first available year post breakup (from 1 to 4 years) for the SHR. For females with living mothers during the settlement pe- riod, we estimated the mother’s home range in the focal bear’s set- tlement year. For females whose mothers died before settlement, we estimated the mother’s home range in the most recently available year (1–2  years before settlement). We extracted the 95% vertices of each annual UD to generate home ranges. We calculated the centroid of the NHR and SHR of each focal individual.

Between 50 and 80% of female bears in the study area are marked on an annual basis (Bellemain et al. 2005; Zedrosser et al.

2006), but not all marked bears have adequate relocation data for home range estimation. To approximate the home ranges of non- focal adult (≥4  years of age) females (n  =  1259 bear years), we used varying methods based on the availability of their location data. We used the available location as an approximated “home range” centroid for each year of the adult lifespan (≥4  years of age) for marked females with only a single geolocation, for ex- ample, the location of their capture or mortality. For marked fe- males with too few locations to estimate a UD, we obtained the centroid of all available points to serve as the annual “home range” centroid. For females with adequate location data, we esti- mated the 95% annual UD and obtained a home range centroid.

We completed a sensitivity analysis to determine the appropriate buffer size around all non-focal female centroids to create as- sumed home ranges. We overlapped circular polygons with radii from 6–15 km with female home ranges from our study popula- tion with the goal of maximizing home range coverage and min- imizing the amount of buffer extending beyond the home range.

A buffer size of 10.5 km was large enough for 95% average cov- erage of all known female home ranges although exceeding home ranges by only 5% on average.

We obtained telemetry data from 56 dispersing females and 31 unique mothers between 1998 and 2018; each represented in the natal and settlement year by ≥20 VHF (range  =  20–151, mean = 80) or ≥1000 GPS (range = 1045–6913, mean = 3543) lo- cations. Local density varied from one to 15 females (mean = 7 ± 4 SD) in the NHR and from zero to 11 females (mean = 5 ± 3) in the SHR (see Table S1 for further variable information).

Defining the social landscape

We identified female focal individuals that had available telemetry data in their natal and settlement years. We overlapped the NHR and SHR of focal individuals with the home ranges of non-focal females in each natal and settlement year (Figure 1). To test for predictions 1 and 3–5, we generated four variables based on the resulting overlap data to represent the social landscape (Figure 1). Maternal overlap is a binary yes/no variable of whether or not the home range of the focal bear’s mother overlapped their own SHR. This variable is only relevant for focal bears who have living mothers in the settlement year. Relatedness ratio in the SHR repre- sents the proportion of related females among all overlapping fe- males. We took tissue and hair samples from captured or dead bears in the population and used 16 microsatellites to genotype 1614 individuals. We constructed pedigrees and used genotypes of individuals to assign relatedness of females (detailed in Frank et al.

2020) with the Lynch–Ritland estimator (Lynch and Ritland 1999).

This method allows for the estimation of the coefficient of relat- edness using molecular markers. We used a pairwise coefficient of relatedness value of ≥0.125 as the threshold for two females to be related. This threshold includes up to third order relationships such as a great grandmother, aunt, or cousin. If all females overlapping the SHR were unrelated, we assigned zero as the relatedness ratio.

We did not include a focal bear’s mother in relatedness ratio due to her being represented already in the maternal overlap variable.

Familiarity index represents females that overlapped the NHR (here- after “familiar”). We divided the number of familiar females by total females overlapping the focal individual’s SHR to generate the familiarity index. If no familiar females overlapped the SHR, we assigned zero. We did not include a focal bear’s mother in the fa- miliarity index due to her existing representation in the maternal overlap variable. Density difference is the difference in local female density (total number of overlapping females) between the SHR and NHR. Obtaining the difference in density allowed us to incor- porate variation in natal density among the focal females, as it rep- resents the change in density from the NHR towards the SHR.

To test prediction 2, that is, the effect of the mother being alive or dead during the settlement period, we overlapped the SHR of the focal female with her mother’s HR in the settlement year or most recent available year. We calculated an index of overlap for a focal female’s SHR and her mother’s HR using the following formula:

Oij/

Ai+Aj

2,

where Oij represents the area of overlap between the mother and focal female’s home ranges, Ai is the total area of the mother’s home range, and Aj is the total area of the focal female’s home range (Støen et  al. 2005); overlap index values are between 0 (no overlap) and 1 (complete overlap).

Statistical analyses and modeling approach

To address our primary objective, the influence of the social en- vironment in settlement decisions, we used second order resource

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/advance-article/doi/10.1093/beheco/arab118/6398953 by Norsk Institutt for Naturforskning, Library user on 22 November 2021

(4)

selection functions (RSF) with a used-available design and an expo- nential selection function (Manly et al. 2002), which represents se- lection at the level of individual home ranges (Johnson 1980). The

‘used’ locations were the set of SHR centroids for the focal females.

For each used location, we generated 5 “available” locations repre- senting potential home range centroids where an individual could have settled (Figure 1). We based availability on empirical dispersal distances from the NHR for our focal females (range 1.3–67.7 km).

We selected 25 km as the upper limit on availability as this rep- resents the 95% distribution of distances between the NHR and SHR and reflects the average dispersal distance for females in this population (Støen et  al. 2006a). We attempted to control for dis- tance by fitting a probability distribution to the dispersal distances and randomly selecting 5 available locations weighted by proba- bility within that buffer using the spsample function in the sp package (Pebesma and Bivand 2005). We buffered each used and available centroid by 10.5 km to generate a series of used and available SHR polygons from which we extracted our social variables (Figure 1).

We used the glmmTMB package (Brooks et al. 2017) to fit a logistic generalized linear mixed model with the binary response variable of used (1) and available (0) SHR. As predictor variables, we used maternal overlap, relatedness ratio, familiarity index, density difference, and a random intercept for focal individual (focalID). We included an additional random intercept for the number of years since family

break up (1–4) to account for variability among individuals. We standardized all continuous predictor variables to have a mean of zero and a standard deviation of one. Due to the importance of habitat features for survival (DeCesare et  al. 2014; Matthiopoulos et  al. 2015), we initially included the following habitat covariates (measured as percent cover over the home range) in our model:

built (anthropogenic) environment, cultivated land, mature forest, young forest, clearcuts, and bogs (see Supplementary S1 for details).

Before modeling, we checked all predictor variables for collin- earity by verifying that all variables had variance inflation factors (VIF) < 2. We conducted further analyses investigating the relation- ship between relatedness ratio and familiarity index because there was potential overlap in those two variables (Supplementary S2). We first fit a base model that included all social predictor variables and no interactions and then sequentially fit the base model containing one interaction term for each combination of predictor variables.

We fit only one interaction term per model due to sample size lim- itations. We did not have a priori hypotheses regarding the relative importance among the social variables, so we fit the base model and fit all possible subsets of the base model using the MuMIn package (Bartoń 2018) and ranked them according to Akaike’s Information Criterion adjusted for small sample sizes (AICc, Hurvich and Tsai 1995). We retained models within ΔAICc ≤ 2 of the highest-ranked model for further assessment and interpretation (Burnham and From natal range:

From settlement range:

Total overlapping females: 3 Total overlapping females: 5 Familiar females from natal range: 2 Related females: 1

Maternal overlap (Yes): 1

Focal female natal range Focal female settlement range Focal female’s mother Familiar female

Unfamiliar, unrelated female Related female

Relatedness ratio: 1/5 = 0.2 Familiarity index: 2/5 = 0.4 Density difference: 5 – 3 = 2 (a) Used and available design

(c) Social environment

(d) Social variables

(b) Home range overlap

Natal

25 km

Used Available

Figure 1

Schematic of how social variables were derived for the study on the influence of social environment on female brown bear settlement home range (SHR) selection. a) Used and available SHR design for the resource selection function. For each used SHR, 5 random available home ranges are created within 25 km of the natal home range. b) A focal female’s natal home range (NHR) and each used and available settlement home range were overlaid with the home ranges of all other females on the landscape. c) Social environment variables were extracted based on the overlapping females, including the density (total overlapping females) in the NHR and SHR, the number of related and known females overlapping the SHR, and whether the focal female’s SHR overlapped her mother’s home range. d) Four social variables (maternal overlap, relatedness ratio, familiarity index, and density difference) were derived based on social environment values extracted in b. Example calculations are given below each (c) and (d).

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/advance-article/doi/10.1093/beheco/arab118/6398953 by Norsk Institutt for Naturforskning, Library user on 22 November 2021

(5)

Anderson 2004). We calculated Nakagawa’s pseudo R2 (Nakagawa and Schielzeth 2013) to evaluate goodness of fit and explanatory power. We averaged coefficients and 95% confidence intervals (CI) over the retained model set for each variable. CIs containing zero were considered uninformative (Arnold 2010).

We employed a bootstrapping modeling procedure to assess rela- tive variable importance. For each permutation, we randomly sam- pled with replacement observations from 90% of the focal bears in the study. In each permutation, we fit the base model and all possible subsets, ranked them via AICc, and retained those with ΔAICc ≤ 2 (Grueber et  al. 2011). We then averaged each set of top models and summed model weights by variable, that is, whether a focal variable was contained in the model. After 1000 permuta- tions, we took the mean value of each variable’s summed Akaike weight to measure the relative importance among the social vari- ables (Galipaud et al. 2014).

To assess how the mother’s presence on the landscape influences settlement decisions, we compared the overlap index between fe- males whose mothers were alive during settlement with those whose mothers died before that period with a Wilcoxon test. One female in each group had overlap values of zero, so we removed them from the analysis to avoid “ties.” We performed all statistical ana- lyses in R 3.6.2 (R Core Team 2019).

RESULTS

We did not detect selection or avoidance for any habitat features at the second order scale and model selection showed the highest sup- port for the model containing only social variables (see Supplement S1 for a detailed description on methodology and results from habitat modeling). We did not detect any informative interactions between the predictor variables (Table S2), and the base model re- ceived greater support than any models containing an interaction term. We also did not see an effect in the random intercept for a year since the family breakup (Table S3) and the base model re- ceived higher support than the model with the additional random intercept.

Two models had ΔAICc ≤ 2; each of the predictor variables appeared in at least one top model (Table 1). Akaike weights of the top models indicated no clear support for a single model, so we present coefficients from the averaged model (Figure 2). Female bears selected for SHR that overlapped mothers (β = 1.03, 95% CI: 0.3–1.67), familiar females (β = 0.8, CI:0.44–

1.17), and areas with higher density relative to their natal range (β  =  0.67, CI: 0.32–1.03), but were indifferent for overlap with related females (β = −0.19, CI: −0.52to 0.15). The results of the model bootstrapping procedure indicated that maternal overlap, den- sity difference, and familiarity index had the highest variable impor- tance (Table 2). In contrast, the CI of relatedness ratio contained zero and had the least relative importance in the bootstrapping procedure (Table 2).

Probability of selection for SHR increased with maternal overlap, presence of familiar females, and areas with higher density relative to their natal range, but not for related females (Figure 3).

Pseudo R2 values for the top models indicated that modeling social factors alone explains 27% of variation in second order settlement patterns for female bears (Table 1).

The overlap index between focal females SHR and their mothers’

home ranges averaged 0.41 ± 0.23 (range 0–0.83). Females whose mothers died before the settlement year (n = 20) had a higher de- gree of overlap with their mother’s last known home range (mean:

0.49 ± 0.24) than females whose mothers were alive (n = 32) in the settlement period (mean: 0.36 ± 0.2, W = 184, p = 0.01, Figure 4).

DISCUSSION

Our study provides evidence that the social environment influ- ences settlement decisions of a large, non-territorial, solitary-living mammal. We found that female brown bears selected SHRs that overlapped their mother’s home range (support P1). When mothers were removed from the landscape, that is, through hunting, fe- male offspring selected SHRs with greater spatial overlap of their mother’s range, as compared with when mothers were alive Table 1

Resource Selection Function model results showing the influence of the social landscape on female brown bear selection of settlement home ranges in Sweden, 1998–2018.

Model selection table shows the two most supported models (highest-ranked model and others ∆AICc < 2) plus the null model. Values shown are the degrees of freedom, log likelihood, AICc, ∆AICc, model weight, and Nakagawa’s Pseudo R2 for the marginal (fixed effects) model. Social variable codes are as follows: densDiff = difference in density from SHR to NHR, famIx = familiarity index, matOver = maternal overlap, relRatio = relatedness ratio

Model set K logLik AICc ΔAICc Wi Pseudo R2 densDiff

+ famIx + matOver

5 −131.72 273.62 0.00 0.6 0.27

densDiff + famIx + matOver + relRatio

6 −131.11 274.48 0.86 0.39 0.27

Null model 2 151.39 306.81 33.2 0 0

Maternal overlap Familiarity index Density difference Relatedness ratio

0.5 0.83

1.97 2.24

2.90

1 2 5 10

Odds ratios Figure 2

Plot of fixed effect coefficient estimates with 95% confidence intervals (coefficients were exponentiated to derive odds ratios) from a resource selection function averaged model estimating the influence of social variables on female brown bear selection of settlement home ranges in Scandinavia (from 1998–2018). Fixed effects were standardized to a mean of zero and a standard deviation of one. Variables with confidence intervals not overlapping one are considered informative. Exponentiated 95%

confidence interval values are 1) maternal overlap (1.51–5.54), 2) familiarity index (1.56–3.22), 3) density difference (1.37–2.82), and 4) relatedness ratio (0.59–1.16).

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/advance-article/doi/10.1093/beheco/arab118/6398953 by Norsk Institutt for Naturforskning, Library user on 22 November 2021

(6)

(support P2). In contrast, we did not find that females selected SHRs that overlapped related females (no support P3). Instead, females selected SHRs that overlapped familiar females (support P4). Lastly, we found evidence that females used density cues when selecting their SHR, but did not select for lower density areas (no support P5).

We found that the strongest predictor of female SHR selection was overlap with the maternal range. In many mammals, females settle in home ranges that overlap their mother’s home range, that is, are philopatric, (Arnaud et  al. 2012; Clutton-Brock and Lukas 2012), likely because resource distribution in that area is most fa- miliar and they receive a certain amount of social tolerance from their mother, as suggested by the resident fitness hypothesis

(Wiggett and Boag 1992). Studies from small rodents demonstrate clear fitness benefits from philopatry: female bushy-tailed woodrats (Neotoma cinerea) who spatially associated with their mothers after in- dependence had increased over-winter survivorship and reduced re- productive failures compared with those that settled in areas lacking their mother (Moses and Millar 1994). Presence of mothers was related to higher survival in female grey mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus), with daughters of present mothers more likely to survive to the breeding season (Lutermann et  al. 2006). Familiarity with resources of an area conveys a considerable advantage to an in- dividual establishing an SHR (Waser and Jones 1983), such as foraging spots (Ashbury et  al. 2020), or refugia from predators (Gehr et al. 2020). For example, female orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) show high spatial overlap with their maternal range from indepen- dence through sexual maturity and benefit from the mother’s high social tolerance and the familiarity with foraging locations (Ashbury et al. 2020). Among Ursids, females often select an SHR that over- laps with their mothers (Powell 1987; McLellan and Hovey 2001;

Støen et al. 2005). However, tolerance is dependent on a mother’s ability to recognize her independent offspring (Waser and Jones 1983). Kin recognition, likely based on olfactory cues (Jojola et al.

2012), has been suggested in our study population (Zedrosser et al.

2007; Swenson and Haroldson 2008). An individual’s mother is the most familiar on the landscape other than full siblings. Thus, it is plausible that female brown bears recognize their independent off- spring and tolerate home range overlap with them.

Our strongest evidence for the influence of the social environ- ment was the difference in settlement patterns based on whether or not an individual’s mother was present and alive on the land- scape during the settlement period. If a focal female’s mother had died before settlement, the settling female established an SHR with greater overlap of her deceased mother’s last known home range.

1.00 (a) (b)

(c) (d)

0.75 0.50 0.25 0.00

1.00 0.75 0.50 0.25 0.00

No

Maternal overlap

Relative probability of selection

Density difference

Familiarity index Relatedness ratio

0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1 Yes

1.00 0.75 0.50 0.25 0.00

1.00 0.75 0.50 0.25 0.00

–14 –10 –6 –2 2

0 0.1 0.2 0.3 0.4 0.5 0.6 0.7 0.8 0.9 1

6 10 14

Figure 3

Relative probability of a female brown bear selecting a settlement home range (SHR) dependent on a) overlapping her mother’s home range, b) the difference in female bear density in the SHR relative to her natal home range, c) the proportion of familiar (individuals a focal female had natal home range overlap with) to total females overlapping the SHR, and d) the proportion of related females to total females overlapping the SHR. Data are predicted from a resource selection model for the female Scandinavian brown bear population between 1998–2018.

Table 2

Results of averaging the top models of the influence of the social landscape on female brown bear settlement home range selection in Scandinavia (between 1998 and 2018).

Summary of parameter estimates, standard error, and 95%

confidence interval (CI) after model averaging each covariate on probability of use for used and available settlement home ranges. Relative importance of each variable is from their summed Akaike weights. Variable codes are as follows:

matOver = maternal overlap, famIx = familiarity index, densDiff = change in density from settlement to natal home range, relRatio = relatedness ratio

Variable β SE CI Relative importance

matOver 1.03 0.32 0.33–1.67 0.95

famIx 0.8 0.19 0.44–1.17 0.99

densDiff 0.67 0.18 0.32–1.03 0.99

relRatio −0.19 0.17 −0.52 to 0.15 0.34

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/advance-article/doi/10.1093/beheco/arab118/6398953 by Norsk Institutt for Naturforskning, Library user on 22 November 2021

(7)

Conversely, for focal females with living mothers in the settlement period, SHR had less overlap with their mother’s range, which in- dicates a mother-offspring conflict over space (Frank et  al. 2017).

Similar patterns have been found in solitary rodents (Lutermann et al. 2006; Arnaud et al. 2012; Sakamoto et al. 2015). For example, experimental removal of Japanese wood mice (Apodemus speciosus) mothers inhibited natal dispersal in their female offspring, whereby females whose mothers were present dispersed more frequently and longer distances (Sakamoto et al. 2015). In American black bears, females partially overlapped their mother’s home range although she was alive, but took over her home range after her death (Powell 1987). Our results suggest a tradeoff in which mothers may limit home range sharing to maximize their own fitness. Parent-offspring conflict theory (Trivers 1974) dictates that conflict is expected be- tween mothers and their independent daughters. A settling female should attempt to maximize her fitness by selecting an SHR with the greatest possible overlap with her mother’s range, providing she incurs no related costs. Delayed primiparity of philopatric females has been shown in this population, indicating a potential cost of selecting an SHR overlapping their mother (Støen et  al. 2006b).

A  young female may be compensated for this cost by increased survival and higher future reproduction, possibly due to famil- iarity with the resources available in their mother’s range. Mothers, however, are expected to limit the amount of overlap to any given daughter to increase her inclusive fitness. A mother that allows her independent daughter greater access to the resources in her HR might decrease her own future survival and reproduction (Trivers 1974). It is possible that mothers are tolerating overlap in marginal areas of their home range although maintaining exclusive use of a

“core” area, thereby minimizing their fitness costs, however, we did not investigate these characteristics within home ranges.

We further found that familiarity but not relatedness was selected for in SHR and no interaction between being related and familiar.

Thus, familiarity alone appears to be a stronger social cue for fe- male bears in settlement decisions. Early associations as juveniles with overlapping females can be important for discrimination of fa- miliar neighbors. Even a short period of association may allow the

recognition of familiar individuals, as shown in Grey seals (Halichoerus grypus) (Robinson et al. 2015). This highlights the possibility of young individuals gathering information on familiar conspecifics that they can use later in life. In ursids, information about the social environ- ment is most likely obtained via scent cues and olfactory commu- nication (Clapham et  al. 2012; Morehouse et  al. 2021), but, given females’ extensive home range overlap, it may also be gained through direct social contact. Future research should directly investigate the use of scent cues and rates of direct contact by female bears.

The benefits of nearby kin have been well documented (Clutton- Brock 2009; Dobson et al. 2012). However, it is becoming increas- ingly clear that familiarity with other individuals, independent of their relatedness, may give fitness benefits (Ylönen et  al. 1990;

Shier and Swaisgood 2012). Higher reproductive success in female great tits (Parus major) was correlated with a higher number of fa- miliar neighbors and whether their nearest neighbor was familiar (Grabowska-Zhang et  al. 2011). Familiarity with conspecifics ap- pears to confer fitness benefits for both survival and reproduction for birds and small mammals (Grabowska-Zhang et al. 2011; Shier and Swaisgood 2012; Siracusa et al. 2021), but it is unknown if this occurs in large mammals. Further research is needed to determine whether settling near closely related or familiar females provides enhanced fitness for dispersing females.

Two potential mechanisms may explain why females select SHR overlapping familiar females but not related females. The first mechanism is the “dear enemy” effect in which an individual will exhibit reduced aggression towards their familiar neighbors com- pared with strangers (Temeles 1994). This is mostly seen in territo- rial species (Rosell and Bjørkøyli 2002; Benten et al. 2020; Vázquez et  al. 2020). Bears are non-territorial, but previous research has indicated that aggression may be a mechanism for explaining the socio-spatial patterns exhibited in brown bears (Støen et al. 2005).

Intraspecific aggression in brown bears has been reported in several populations across their geographic range (Miller 1985; McLellan 1994; Swenson et  al. 2001). If aggressive encounters are reduced by selecting SHR overlapping familiar neighbors, this benefits the focal female. Brown bear females exhibit strong site fidelity and stable home ranges are conducive to establishing familiarity with neighboring females. The second mechanism is through familiarity- based kin recognition, that is, discrimination of kin based on as- sociation or familiarity (Tang-Martinez 2001). This alternative to phenotypic kin recognition is location-dependent; if an individual is adjacent, they are likely related (Mateo 2004). Tolerance behavior towards neighboring females can be maintained in populations that exhibit philopatry due to the probability that neighbors are closely related (Waser and Jones 1983). If familiars are also relatives, there could be an increase in inclusive fitness. Due to the generally phil- opatric nature of female brown bears, related individuals are typi- cally clustered spatially (Støen et al. 2005). Despite the suggestion of kin recognition in our population, our results do not suggest its occurrence among female brown bears, but there is support for in- dividual recognition through prior association.

We predicted that females would detect density differences on the landscape and select SHR in areas of lower density, as sug- gested by previous research (Støen et  al. 2006a); however, we did not find support for that in our study. Although females did appear to respond to density in this study population, contrary to our pre- diction, they selected SHR in areas with higher density relative to their natal ranges. Females possibly settle in areas with higher female density because it could indicate higher quality habitat.

Although male bears may disperse into areas lacking conspecifics (Wiggett and Boag 1992). Studies from small rodents demonstrate

clear fitness benefits from philopatry: female bushy-tailed woodrats (Neotoma cinerea) who spatially associated with their mothers after in- dependence had increased over-winter survivorship and reduced re- productive failures compared with those that settled in areas lacking their mother (Moses and Millar 1994). Presence of mothers was related to higher survival in female grey mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus), with daughters of present mothers more likely to survive to the breeding season (Lutermann et  al. 2006). Familiarity with resources of an area conveys a considerable advantage to an in- dividual establishing an SHR (Waser and Jones 1983), such as foraging spots (Ashbury et  al. 2020), or refugia from predators (Gehr et al. 2020). For example, female orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) show high spatial overlap with their maternal range from indepen- dence through sexual maturity and benefit from the mother’s high social tolerance and the familiarity with foraging locations (Ashbury et al. 2020). Among Ursids, females often select an SHR that over- laps with their mothers (Powell 1987; McLellan and Hovey 2001;

Støen et al. 2005). However, tolerance is dependent on a mother’s ability to recognize her independent offspring (Waser and Jones 1983). Kin recognition, likely based on olfactory cues (Jojola et al.

2012), has been suggested in our study population (Zedrosser et al.

2007; Swenson and Haroldson 2008). An individual’s mother is the most familiar on the landscape other than full siblings. Thus, it is plausible that female brown bears recognize their independent off- spring and tolerate home range overlap with them.

Our strongest evidence for the influence of the social environ- ment was the difference in settlement patterns based on whether or not an individual’s mother was present and alive on the land- scape during the settlement period. If a focal female’s mother had died before settlement, the settling female established an SHR with greater overlap of her deceased mother’s last known home range.

1.00

0.75

0.50

0.25

0.00

Alive

Status of mother

Overlap index

Dead

Figure 4

Violin plot showing relationship between the amount of overlap between the settlement home range of a female brown bear and the home range of her mother. The x axis indicates whether the mother of the female was alive during the settlement period or had died before settlement. Overlap index values on the y axis are between 0 and 1.  Mean overlap for females with living mothers was 0.36 (±0.2); mean overlap for females whose mother died before settlement was 0.49 (±0.24).

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/advance-article/doi/10.1093/beheco/arab118/6398953 by Norsk Institutt for Naturforskning, Library user on 22 November 2021

(8)

(Zedrosser et al. 2007), this is not seen with females. Additional re- search investigating the relationship between population density and habitat quality could help shed light on this settlement pattern.

Strikingly, females do not appear to use non-social environmental cues (i.e., habitat types) when selecting SHR at the second order.

Human-mediated homogenization of landscapes through large scale forestry in the study area has created consistent cut blocks and regenerating stands (Josefsson et al. 2010). This suggests that impor- tant heterogeneity cues for settlement decisions occurs within the social landscape. Changes in the social makeup of this population are largely driven by hunting (Gosselin et  al. 2015; Bischof et  al.

2018). As adult females are removed from the population via har- vest, surviving females will shift their home ranges to “fill in” va- cancies left by the deceased female (Frank et al. 2017). This annual variation in the distribution of the population would make sensi- tivity towards and use of cues regarding the social environment particularly valuable not only for settlement decisions but also when expanding or shifting the home range configuration over time. Our study highlights that the social environment, beyond conspecific presence or density, is an important consideration when describing settlement decisions and dispersal patterns, and that such informa- tion is important for solitary-living species.

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL

Supplementary data are available at Behavioral Ecology online.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

We are grateful to the many field workers and volunteers that have contrib- uted to the data collection for this study during the course of the years. We especially acknowledge the help of Dr. honoris causa S. Brunberg. We would like to thank three anonymous reviewers whose feedback has greatly im- proved the manuscript. The long-term funding of the Scandinavian Brown Bear Research Project (SBBRP) has come primarily from the Swedish Environmental Protection Agency, the Norwegian Environment Agency, the Austrian Science Fund, and the Swedish Association for Hunting and Wildlife Management. This is paper No. 318 from the SBBRP.

FUNDING

This study was funded through the 2015–2016 BiodivERsA COFUND call for research proposals, with the national funders Agence Nationale de la Recherche (France) (ANR-16-EBI3-0003), National Science Center (Poland) (2016/22/Z/NZ8/00121), Project Management Agency of the German Aerospace Center (01LC1614A), Romanian National Authority for Scientific Research and Innovation (BiodivERsA3-2015-147-BearConnect (96/2016), and The Research Council of Norway (269863). AGH has received funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement no. 793077.

Data availability: Analyses reported in this article can be reproduced using the data provided by Hansen et al. (2021).

Handling editor: John Quinn

REFERENCES

Armansin NC, Stow AJ, Cantor M, Leu ST, Klarevas-Irby JA, Chariton AA, Farine DR. 2020. Social barriers in ecological landscapes: the social re- sistance hypothesis. Trends Ecol Evol. 35:137–148.

Arnaud  CM, Dobson  FS, Murie  JO. 2012. Philopatry and within-colony movements in Columbian ground squirrels. Mol Ecol. 21:493–504.

Arnemo JM, Evans A. 2017. Biomedical protocols for free-ranging brown bears, wolves, wolverines and lynx. Technical report. Evenstad, Norway:

Inland Norway University of Applied Sciences.

Arnold  TW. 2010. Uninformative parameters and model selection using Akaike’s Information Criterion. J Wildl Manage. 74:1175–1178.

Ashbury  AM, Willems  EP, Utami  Atmoko  SS, Saputra  F, van  Schaik  CP, van Noordwijk MA. 2020. Home range establishment and the mechan- isms of philopatry among female Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii) at Tuanan. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 74:42.

Barton NH. 1992. The genetic consequences of dispersal. In: Stenseth NC, Lidicker  WZ, editors. Animal dispersal: small mammals as a model.

Dordrecht: Springer Science & Business Media. p. 37–59.

Bartoń K. 2018. MuMIn: multi-model inference. R package version 1.42.1.

Cran-R. 1:289–290.

Bellemain  E, Swenson  JE, Tallmon  D, Brunberg  S, Taberlet  P. 2005.

Estimating population size of elusive animals with DNA from hunter- collected feces: four methods for brown bears. Conserv Biol. 19:150–161.

Benard MF, McCauley SJ. 2008. Integrating across life-history stages: con- sequences of natal habitat effects on dispersal. Am Nat. 171:553–567.

Benten A, Cross HB, Tinnesand VH, Zedrosser A, Rosell F. 2020. Distant neighbours: friends or foes? Eurasian beavers show context-dependent re- sponses to simulated intruders. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 74:1–10.

Bischof  R, Bonenfant  C, Rivrud  IM, Zedrosser  A, Friebe  A, Coulson  T, Mysterud A, Swenson JE. 2018. Regulated hunting re-shapes the life his- tory of brown bears. Nat Ecol Evol. 2:116–123.

Bowler  DE, Benton  TG. 2005. Causes and consequences of animal dis- persal strategies: relating individual behaviour to spatial dynamics. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 80:205–225.

Brooks  ME, Kristensen  K, van  Benthem  KJ, Magnusson  A, Berg  CW, Nielsen A, Skaug HJ, Machler M, Bolker BM. 2017. glmmTMB balances speed and flexibility among packages for zero-inflated generalized linear mixed modeling. R J. 9:378–400.

Burnham  KP, Anderson  DR. 2004. Multimodel inference: understanding AIC and BIC in model selection. Sociol Methods Res. 33:261–304.

Calenge  C. 2006. The package “adehabitat” for the R software: a tool for the analysis of space and habitat use by animals. Ecol Model.

197:516–519.

Clapham M, Nevin OT, Ramsey AD, Rosell F. 2012. A hypothetico-deduc- tive approach to assessing the social function of chemical signalling in a non-territorial solitary carnivore. PLoS One. 7:e35404.

Clapham M, Nevin OT, Ramsey AD, Rosell F. 2014. Scent-marking invest- ment and motor patterns are affected by the age and sex of wild brown bears. Anim Behav. 94:107–116.

Clobert  J, Danchin  É, Dhondt  AA, Nicholds  JD. 2001. Dispersal. New York: Oxford University Press.

Clutton-Brock  T. 2009. Structure and function in mammalian societies.

Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 364:3229–3242.

Clutton-Brock TH, Lukas D. 2012. The evolution of social philopatry and dispersal in female mammals. Mol Ecol. 21:472–492.

Cote  J, Clobert  J. 2010. Risky dispersal: avoiding kin competition despite uncertainty. Ecology. 91:1485–1493.

D’eon RG, Delparte D. 2005. Effects of radio-collar position and orienta- tion on GPS radio-collar performance, and the implications of PDOP in data screening. J Appl Ecol. 42:383–388.

Dahle  B, Swenson  JE. 2003. Home ranges in adult Scandinavian brown bears (Ursus arctos): effect of mass, sex, reproductive category, population density and habitat type. J Zool. 260:329–335.

Dall  SR, Giraldeau  LA, Olsson  O, McNamara  JM, Stephens  DW. 2005.

Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. Trends Ecol Evol. 20:187–193.

Danchin E, Giraldeau LA, Valone TJ, Wagner RH. 2004. Public informa- tion: from nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science. 305:487–491.

Danchin É, Heg D, Doligez B. 2001. Public information and breeding hab- itat selection. In: Clobert J, Danchin É, Dhondt AA, Nichols JD, editors.

Dispersal. New York: Oxford University Press. p. 243–258.

DeCesare  NJ, Hebblewhite  M, Bradley  M, Hervieux  D, Neufeld  L, Musiani M. 2014. Linking habitat selection and predation risk to spatial variation in survival. J Anim Ecol. 83:343–352.

Dobson  FS, Viblanc  VA, Arnaud  CM, Murie  JO. 2012. Kin selection in Columbian ground squirrels: direct and indirect fitness benefits. Mol Ecol. 21:524–531.

Doligez  B, Danchin  E, Clobert  J. 2002. Public information and breeding habitat selection in a wild bird population. Science. 297:1168–1170.

Frank  SC, Leclerc  M, Pelletier  F, Rosell  F, Swenson  JE, Bischof  R, Kindberg J, Eiken HG, Hagen SB, Zedrosser A. 2017. Sociodemographic factors modulate the spatial response of brown bears to vacancies created by hunting. J Anim Ecol. 87:247–258.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/advance-article/doi/10.1093/beheco/arab118/6398953 by Norsk Institutt for Naturforskning, Library user on 22 November 2021

(9)

Frank  SC, Pelletier  F, Kopatz  A, Bourret  A, Garant  D, Swenson  JE, Eiken HG, Hagen SB, Zedrosser A. 2020. Harvest is associated with the disruption of social and fine-scale genetic structure among matrilines of a solitary large carnivore. Evol Appl. 1–13. doi: 10.1111/eva.13178.

Frank  SC, Steyaert  SM, Swenson  JE, Storch  I, Kindberg  J, Barck  H, Zedrosser  A. 2015. A “clearcut” case? Brown bear selection of coarse woody debris and carpenter ants on clearcuts. For Ecol Manage.

348:164–173.

Fretwell  SD, Lucas  HL. 1969. On territorial behavior and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds. Acta Biotheor. 19:16–36.

Friebe A, Swenson JE, Sandegren F. 2001. Denning chronology of female brown bears in central Sweden. Ursus. 12:37–45.

Galipaud  M, Gillingham  MA, David  M, Dechaume-Moncharmont  FX.

2014. Ecologists overestimate the importance of predictor variables in model averaging: a plea for cautious interpretations. Methods Ecol Evol.

5:983–991.

Gehr  B, Bonnot  NC, Heurich  M, Cagnacci  F, Ciuti  S, Hewison  AJM, Galliard  J-M, Ranc  N, Premier  J, Vogt  K, et  al. 2020. Stay home, stay safe—Site familiarity reduces predation risk in a large herbivore in two contrasting study sites. J Anim Ecol. 89:1329–1339.

Gosselin J, Zedrosser A, Swenson JE, Pelletier F. 2015. The relative impor- tance of direct and indirect effects of hunting mortality on the popula- tion dynamics of brown bears. Proc Biol Sci. 282:20141840.

Grabowska-Zhang AM, Wilkin TA, Sheldon BC. 2011. Effects of neighbor familiarity on reproductive success in the great tit (Parus major). Behav Ecol. 23:322–333.

Grueber  CE, Nakagawa  S, Laws  RJ, Jamieson  IG. 2011. Multimodel in- ference in ecology and evolution: challenges and solutions. J Evol Biol.

24:699–711.

Hansen  JE, Hertel  AH, Frank  SC, Kindberg  J, Zedrosser  A. 2021. Social environment shapes female settlement decisions in a solitary carnivore.

Behav Ecol. doi: 10.5061/dryad.8pk0p2np5.

Hare  JF, Campbell  KL, Senkiw  RW. 2014. Catch the wave: prairie dogs assess neighbours’ awareness using contagious displays. Proc Biol Sci.

281:20132153.

Hurvich CM, Tsai CL. 1995. Model selection for extended quasi-likelihood models in small samples. Biometrics. 51:1077–1084.

Johnson  DH. 1980. The comparison of usage and availability measure- ments for evaluating resource preference. Ecology. 61:65–71.

Jojola SM, Rosell F, Warrington I, Swenson JE, Zedrosser A. 2012. Subadult brown bears (Ursus arctos) discriminate between unfamiliar adult male and female anal gland secretion. Mamm Biol. 77:363–368.

Josefsson  T, Gunnarson  B, Liedgren  L, Bergman  I, Östlund  L. 2010.

Historical human influence on forest composition and structure in boreal Fennoscandia. Can J For Res. 40:872–884.

Lawson  Handley  LJ, Perrin  N. 2007. Advances in our understanding of mammalian sex-biased dispersal. Mol Ecol. 16:1559–1578.

Lutermann H, Schmelting B, Radespiel U, Ehresmann P, Zimmermann E.

2006. The role of survival for the evolution of female philopatry in a solitary forager, the grey mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus). Proc Biol Sci.

273:2527–2533.

Lynch M, Ritland K. 1999. Estimation of pairwise relatedness with molec- ular markers. Genetics. 152:1753–1766.

Mace  RD, Waller  JS. 1997. Spatial and temporal interaction of male and female grizzly bears in Northwestern Montana. J Wildl Manage.

61:39–52.

Manly  BF, McDonald  L, Thomas  D, McDonald  TL, Erickson  WP. 2002.

Resource selection by animals: statistical design and analysis for field studies. 2nd ed. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

Mateo  JM. 2002. Kin-recognition abilities and nepotism as a function of sociality. Proc Biol Sci. 269:721–727.

Mateo  JM. 2004. Recognition systems and biological organization:

the perception component of social recognition. Ann Zool Fenn.

41:729–745.

Matthiopoulos J, Fieberg J, Aarts G, Beyer HL, Morales JM, Haydon DT.

2015. Establishing the link between habitat selection and animal popula- tion dynamics. Ecol Monogr. 85:413–436.

Matthysen  E. 2005. Density-dependent dispersal in birds and mammals.

Ecography. 28:403–416.

McLellan  BN. 1994. Density-dependent population regulation of brown bears. International conference on bear research and management, mon- ograph series no. 3:15–24.

McLellan  BN, Hovey  FW. 2001. Natal dispersal of grizzly bears. Can J Zool. 79:838–844.

Miller  SD. 1985. An observation of inter- and intra-specific aggression involving brown bear, black bear, and moose in Southcentral Alaska. J Mammal. 66:805–806.

Morehouse AT, Loosen AE, Graves TA, Boyce MS. 2021. The smell of suc- cess: reproductive success related to rub behavior in brown bears. PLoS One. 16:e0247964.

Moses  RA, Millar  JS. 1994. Philopatry and mother-daughter associations in bushy-tailed woodrats: space use and reproductive success. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 35:131–140.

Nakagawa  S, Schielzeth  H. 2013. A general and simple method for obtaining R2 from generalized linear mixed-effects models. Methods Ecol Evol. 4:133–142.

Nocera  JJ, Forbes  GJ, Giraldeau  LA. 2006. Inadvertent social informa- tion in breeding site selection of natal dispersing birds. Proc Biol Sci.

273:349–355.

O’Mara MT, Dechmann DKN, Page RA. 2014. Frugivorous bats evaluate the quality of social information when choosing novel foods. Behav Ecol.

25:1233–1239.

Ordiz A, Støen O-G, Sæbø S, Kindberg J, Delibes M, Swenson JE. 2012.

Do bears know they are being hunted? Biol Conserv. 152:21–28.

Ordiz  A, Støen  OG, Swenson  JE, Kojola  I, Bischof  R. 2008. Distance- dependent effect of the nearest neighbor: spatiotemporal patterns in brown bear reproduction. Ecology. 89:3327–3335.

Pebesma E, Bivand RS. 2005. S classes and methods for spatial data: the sp package. R News. 5:9–13.

Powell RA. 1987. Black bear home range overlap in North Carolina and the concept of home range applied to black bears. Int C Bear. 7:235–242.

Pulliam  HR, Danielson  BJ. 1991. Sources, sinks, and habitat selection: a landscape perspective on population dynamics. Am Nat. 137:S50–S66.

R Core Team. 2019. R: a language and environment for statistical com- puting. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing.

Revilla  E, Ramos  Fernández  D, Fernández-Gil  A, Sergiel  A, Selva  N, Naves J. 2021. Brown bear communication hubs: patterns and correlates of tree rubbing and pedal marking at a long-term marking site. PeerJ.

9:e10447.

Robinson  BG, Larsen  KW, Kerr  HJ. 2011. Natal experience and conspe- cifics influence the settling behaviour of the juvenile terrestrial isopod Armadillidium vulgare. Can J Zool. 89:661–667.

Robinson KJ, Twiss SD, Hazon N, Moss S, Lonergan M, Pomeroy PP. 2015.

Conspecific recognition and aggression reduction to familiars in newly weaned, socially plastic mammals. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 69:1383–1394.

Rosell F, Bjørkøyli T. 2002. A test of the dear enemy phenomenon in the Eurasian beaver. Anim Behav. 63:1073–1078.

Sakamoto  SH, Eto  T, Okubo  Y, Shinohara  A, Morita  T, Koshimoto  C.

2015. The effects of maternal presence on natal dispersal are seasonally flexible in an asocial rodent. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 69:1075–1084.

Sergiel A, Naves J, Kujawski P, Maślak R, Serwa E, Ramos D, Fernández- Gil  A, Revilla  E, Zwijacz-Kozica  T, Zięba  F, et  al. 2017. Histological, chemical and behavioural evidence of pedal communication in brown bears. Sci Rep. 7:1052.

Shier DM, Swaisgood RR. 2012. Fitness costs of neighborhood disruption in translocations of a solitary mammal. Conserv Biol. 26:116–123.

Siracusa ER, Boutin S, Dantzer B, Lane JE, Coltman DW, McAdam AG.

2021. Familiar neighbors, but not relatives, enhance fitness in a territorial mammal. Curr Biol. 31:438–445.e3.

Stamps JA. 2001. Habitat selection by dispersers: integrating proximate and ultimate approaches. In: Clobert J, Danchin E, Dhondt AA, Nichols JD, editors. Dispersal. New York: Oxford University Press. p. 230–242.

Stamps JA, Davis JM. 2006. Adaptive effects of natal experience on habitat selection by dispersers. Anim Behav. 72:1279–1289.

Steyaert  SM, Endrestøl  A, Hacklaender  K, Swenson  JE, Zedrosser  A.

2012. The mating system of the brown bear Ursus arctos. Mamm Rev.

42:12–34.

Stockley P, Bro-Jørgensen J. 2011. Female competition and its evolutionary consequences in mammals. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 86:341–366.

Støen  O-G, Bellemain  E, Sæbø  S, Swenson  JE. 2005. Kin-related spa- tial structure in brown bears Ursus arctos. Behav Ecol Sociobiol.

59:191–197.

Støen  OG, Zedrosser  A, Saebø  S, Swenson  JE. 2006a. Inversely density- dependent natal dispersal in brown bears Ursus arctos. Oecologia.

148:356–364.

Støen  O-G, Zedrosser  A, Wegge  P, Swenson  JE. 2006b. Socially induced delayed primiparity in brown bears Ursus arctos. Behav Ecol Sociobiol.

61:1–8.

Arnold  TW. 2010. Uninformative parameters and model selection using Akaike’s Information Criterion. J Wildl Manage. 74:1175–1178.

Ashbury  AM, Willems  EP, Utami  Atmoko  SS, Saputra  F, van  Schaik  CP, van Noordwijk MA. 2020. Home range establishment and the mechan- isms of philopatry among female Bornean orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus wurmbii) at Tuanan. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 74:42.

Barton NH. 1992. The genetic consequences of dispersal. In: Stenseth NC, Lidicker  WZ, editors. Animal dispersal: small mammals as a model.

Dordrecht: Springer Science & Business Media. p. 37–59.

Bartoń K. 2018. MuMIn: multi-model inference. R package version 1.42.1.

Cran-R. 1:289–290.

Bellemain  E, Swenson  JE, Tallmon  D, Brunberg  S, Taberlet  P. 2005.

Estimating population size of elusive animals with DNA from hunter- collected feces: four methods for brown bears. Conserv Biol. 19:150–161.

Benard MF, McCauley SJ. 2008. Integrating across life-history stages: con- sequences of natal habitat effects on dispersal. Am Nat. 171:553–567.

Benten A, Cross HB, Tinnesand VH, Zedrosser A, Rosell F. 2020. Distant neighbours: friends or foes? Eurasian beavers show context-dependent re- sponses to simulated intruders. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 74:1–10.

Bischof  R, Bonenfant  C, Rivrud  IM, Zedrosser  A, Friebe  A, Coulson  T, Mysterud A, Swenson JE. 2018. Regulated hunting re-shapes the life his- tory of brown bears. Nat Ecol Evol. 2:116–123.

Bowler  DE, Benton  TG. 2005. Causes and consequences of animal dis- persal strategies: relating individual behaviour to spatial dynamics. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc. 80:205–225.

Brooks  ME, Kristensen  K, van  Benthem  KJ, Magnusson  A, Berg  CW, Nielsen A, Skaug HJ, Machler M, Bolker BM. 2017. glmmTMB balances speed and flexibility among packages for zero-inflated generalized linear mixed modeling. R J. 9:378–400.

Burnham  KP, Anderson  DR. 2004. Multimodel inference: understanding AIC and BIC in model selection. Sociol Methods Res. 33:261–304.

Calenge  C. 2006. The package “adehabitat” for the R software: a tool for the analysis of space and habitat use by animals. Ecol Model.

197:516–519.

Clapham M, Nevin OT, Ramsey AD, Rosell F. 2012. A hypothetico-deduc- tive approach to assessing the social function of chemical signalling in a non-territorial solitary carnivore. PLoS One. 7:e35404.

Clapham M, Nevin OT, Ramsey AD, Rosell F. 2014. Scent-marking invest- ment and motor patterns are affected by the age and sex of wild brown bears. Anim Behav. 94:107–116.

Clobert  J, Danchin  É, Dhondt  AA, Nicholds  JD. 2001. Dispersal. New York: Oxford University Press.

Clutton-Brock  T. 2009. Structure and function in mammalian societies.

Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 364:3229–3242.

Clutton-Brock TH, Lukas D. 2012. The evolution of social philopatry and dispersal in female mammals. Mol Ecol. 21:472–492.

Cote  J, Clobert  J. 2010. Risky dispersal: avoiding kin competition despite uncertainty. Ecology. 91:1485–1493.

D’eon RG, Delparte D. 2005. Effects of radio-collar position and orienta- tion on GPS radio-collar performance, and the implications of PDOP in data screening. J Appl Ecol. 42:383–388.

Dahle  B, Swenson  JE. 2003. Home ranges in adult Scandinavian brown bears (Ursus arctos): effect of mass, sex, reproductive category, population density and habitat type. J Zool. 260:329–335.

Dall  SR, Giraldeau  LA, Olsson  O, McNamara  JM, Stephens  DW. 2005.

Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. Trends Ecol Evol. 20:187–193.

Danchin E, Giraldeau LA, Valone TJ, Wagner RH. 2004. Public informa- tion: from nosy neighbors to cultural evolution. Science. 305:487–491.

Danchin É, Heg D, Doligez B. 2001. Public information and breeding hab- itat selection. In: Clobert J, Danchin É, Dhondt AA, Nichols JD, editors.

Dispersal. New York: Oxford University Press. p. 243–258.

DeCesare  NJ, Hebblewhite  M, Bradley  M, Hervieux  D, Neufeld  L, Musiani M. 2014. Linking habitat selection and predation risk to spatial variation in survival. J Anim Ecol. 83:343–352.

Dobson  FS, Viblanc  VA, Arnaud  CM, Murie  JO. 2012. Kin selection in Columbian ground squirrels: direct and indirect fitness benefits. Mol Ecol. 21:524–531.

Doligez  B, Danchin  E, Clobert  J. 2002. Public information and breeding habitat selection in a wild bird population. Science. 297:1168–1170.

Frank  SC, Leclerc  M, Pelletier  F, Rosell  F, Swenson  JE, Bischof  R, Kindberg J, Eiken HG, Hagen SB, Zedrosser A. 2017. Sociodemographic factors modulate the spatial response of brown bears to vacancies created by hunting. J Anim Ecol. 87:247–258.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/advance-article/doi/10.1093/beheco/arab118/6398953 by Norsk Institutt for Naturforskning, Library user on 22 November 2021

(10)

Swenson  JE, Dahle  B, Sandegren  F. 2001. Intraspecific predation in Scandinavian brown bears older than cubs-of-the-year. Ursus.

12:81–91.

Swenson  JE, Haroldson  MA. 2008. Observations of mixed-aged litters in brown bears. Ursus. 19:73–79.

Swenson  JE, Sandegren  F, Bjärvall  A, Söderberg  A, Wabakken  P, Franzén  R. 1994. Size, trend, distribution and conservation of the brown bear Ursus arctos population in Sweden. Biol Conserv.

70:9–17.

Tang-Martinez Z. 2001. The mechanisms of kin discrimination and the ev- olution of kin recognition in vertebrates: a critical re-evaluation. Behav Processes. 53:21–40.

Temeles EJ. 1994. The role of neighbours in territorial systems: when are they “dear enemies”? Anim Behav. 47:339–350.

Trivers RL. 1974. Parent-offspring conflict. Am Zool. 14:249–264.

Valone TJ. 2007. From eavesdropping on performance to copying the beha- vior of others: a review of public information use. Behav Ecol Sociobiol.

62:1–14.

Van  de  Walle  J, Pigeon  G, Zedrosser  A, Swenson  JE, Pelletier  F. 2018.

Hunting regulation favors slow life histories in a large carnivore. Nat Commun. 9:1–10.

Vázquez  J, Fargallo  JA, Jiménez  N, Aguilar-Montiel  F, Rodríguez- Martínez  L. 2020. Dear enemy effect in the Mexican volcano mouse

Neotomodon alstoni: implications of sex in the agonistic behaviour among neighbours. Behav Processes. 181:104251.

Vercken E, Sinervo B, Clobert J. 2012. The importance of a good neighbor- hood: dispersal decisions in juvenile common lizards are based on social environment. Behav Ecol. 23:1059–1067.

Waser PM, Jones WT. 1983. Natal philopatry among solitary mammals. Q Rev Biol. 58:355–390.

Wey TW, Spiegel O, Montiglio P-O, Mabry KE. 2015. Natal dispersal in a social landscape: considering individual behavioral phenotypes and social environment in dispersal ecology. Curr Zool. 61:543–556.

Wiggett D, Boag DA. 1992. The resident fitness hypothesis and dispersal by yearling female Columbian ground squirrels. Can J Zool. 70:1984–1994.

Ylönen  H, Mappes  T, Viitala  J. 1990. Different demography of friends and strangers: an experiment on the impact of kinship and familiarity in Clethrionomys glareolus. Oecologia. 83:333–337.

Zedrosser  A, Dahle  B, Støen  OG, Swenson  JE. 2009. The effects of primiparity on reproductive performance in the brown bear. Oecologia.

160:847–854.

Zedrosser A, Dahle B, Swenson JE. 2006. Population density and food con- ditions determine adult female body size in brown bears. J Mammal.

87:510–518.

Zedrosser A, Støen OG, Sæbø S, Swenson JE. 2007. Should I stay or should I go? Natal dispersal in the brown bear. Anim Behav. 74:369–376.

Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/beheco/advance-article/doi/10.1093/beheco/arab118/6398953 by Norsk Institutt for Naturforskning, Library user on 22 November 2021

Referanser

RELATERTE DOKUMENTER

The Scandinavian States are leading within all UN member states (193) by most of socio- economic surrounding parameters characterising the transformations aiming to the UN SDG

A UAV will reduce the hop count for long flows, increasing the efficiency of packet forwarding, allowing for improved network throughput. On the other hand, the potential for

What benefits can this model provide? It seems clear that using a socio-technical perspective and introducing the concept of online information sediments to describe social

This report presented effects of cultural differences in individualism/collectivism, power distance, uncertainty avoidance, masculinity/femininity, and long term/short

Preliminary numerical simulation of the dispersion of chlorine vapour in a mock urban environment for the Jack Rabbit II

Potential individual perceived barriers to using the SMART concept are being understood by analyzing how different factors that hinder and promote the motivation to use SMART

By means of analysing a photograph like the one presented here, it can be seen that major physical and social changes have taken place in the course of a time as short as 13

These commitments lie in the social life of workers outside and around the cooperative environment, a social life including kinship and friend- ship relations, memories of